#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Inter-method and anatomical correlates of episodic memory tests in the Alzheimer’s Disease spectrum


Authors: Felipe Kenji Sudo aff001;  Andrea Silveira de Souza aff001;  Claudia Drummond aff001;  Naima Assuncao aff001;  Alina Teldeschi aff001;  Natalia Oliveira aff001;  Fernanda Rodrigues aff001;  Gustavo Santiago-Bravo aff001;  Victor Calil aff001;  Gabriel Lima aff001;  Pilar Erthal aff001;  Gabriel Bernardes aff001;  Marina Monteiro aff001;  Fernanda Tovar-Moll aff001;  Paulo Mattos aff001
Authors place of work: D’Or Institute for Research and Education, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil aff001;  Department of Speech and Hearing Pathology, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil aff002;  Morphological Sciences Program, Institute of Biomedical Sciences, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil aff003;  Post-Graduation Program in Clinical Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil aff004;  Institute of Psychiatry, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil aff005
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 14(10)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223731

Summary

Background

Episodic memory impairments have been described as initial clinical findings in the Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) spectrum, which could be associated with the presence of early hippocampal dysfunction. However, correlates between performances in neuropsychological tests and hippocampal volumes in AD were inconclusive in the literature. Divergent methods to assess episodic memory have been depicted as a major source of heterogeneity across studies.

Methods

We examined correlates among performances in three different delayed-recall tasks (Rey-Auditory Verbal-Learning Test–RAVLT, Logical Memory and Visual Reproduction subtests from the Wechsler Memory Scale) and fully-automated volumetric measurements of the hippocampus (estimated using Neuroquant®) of 83 older subjects (47 controls, 27 Mild Cognitive Impairment individuals and 9 participants with Dementia due to AD).

Results

Inter-method correlations of episodic memory performances were at most moderate. Scores in the RAVLT predicted up to 48% of variance in HOC (Hippocampal Occupancy Score) among subjects in the AD spectrum.

Discussion

Tests using different stimuli (verbal or visual) and presenting distinct designs (word list, story or figure learning) may assess divergent aspects in episodic memory, with heterogeneous anatomical correlates.

Conclusions

Different episodic memory tests might not assess the same construct and should not be used interchangeably. Scores in RAVLT may correlate with the presence of neurodegeneration in AD.

Keywords:

Learning – Cognitive impairment – Alzheimer's disease – memory – Vision – Neuropsychological testing – Memory recall – Hippocampus

Introduction

Episodic memory refers to human’s ability to consciously recollect situations and events through the effective acquisition, retention and recall of verbal and visuospatial data[1]. From a neural perspective, a rich body of evidence concerning the pivotal role of the hippocampus for the space-time organization and storage of information has been provided by studies with geriatric population, especially those focused on the Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) spectrum[2]. In those cases, it has become accepted that hippocampal dysfunction occurs during the course of the disorder, following many years of asymptomatic parenchymal accumulation of β-amyloid peptides and tau protein[3]. With the escalation of this neuropathological process, the clinical stage of the disorder initiates, typically manifesting as early episodic memory impairments[3,4].

Regarding the current knowledge about the pathophysiology of AD, episodic memory testing and structural neuroimaging remain relevant for the assessment of older subjects with suspected cognitive impairment[5]. However, findings on the correlates between cognitive and AD-related anatomical features are largely discordant, which might be attributed to the complex nature of episodic memory itself, as well as to inconsistencies across methods for neuropsychological assessment[4] and brain volume estimation[6,7]. For instance, episodic memory tasks may apply stimuli of different materials (verbal or visuospatial, for example) or they may assess distinct components within this cognitive ability (acquisition, retention and delayed-recall)[4]. Moreover, measurement models range from evaluating memory capacity for semantically uncorrelated items (verbal item-memory tasks, such as word lists[8]) to investigating recollection of sequences of logically-linked ideas (for example, story learning)[9]. Thus, discrepant episodic memory performances across studies could be interpreted as effects of samples with different levels of cognitive impairment or as influence of disproportional task-specific demands[10,11].

Likewise, an array of strategies has been described in the literature for the evaluation of medial temporal cortex atrophy in AD, such as visual rating scales and computer-based methods, namely manual, semi-automated and automated volumetric measurements[12]. Translating brain volumetrics into clinical practice has been hampered by many factors, such as the high cost of the instruments, the time-consuming processing operations, the lack of harmonized approaches across laboratories and the paucity of normative data for grey and white matter volumes among older population [11,13,14]. As an effort to overcome those limitations, NeuroQuant®, an FDA approved software for automatic labeling, visualization and volumetric quantification of brain structures, was commercially released by CorTechs Laboratories in 2007. This method has been cross-validated with manual segmentation[15] and with other well-known brain morphometry procedures in AD samples (FreeSurfer, for example)[16]. In addition, neuroimaging parameters in NeuroQuant® for each subject have been compared to an extensive and continuously growing cloud-based normative database[17].

Analyzing correlations across different episodic memory tasks would allow inferring about whether those instruments could be employed interchangeably. Specifically, it would clarify about the construct validity of those neuropsychological tests. In addition, according to studies, hippocampal volume appears to be the strongest individual predictor of short-term cognitive decline in older population, compared to other AD biomarkers[18]. Estimating associations between scores in memory tests and brain volumes could indicate which cognitive index best reflects neurodegeneration in subjects within the AD spectrum. Therefore, the present study aimed at investigating: (1) the convergent validity of different measures of episodic memory; and (2) the clinical-anatomical correlation between memory performances and volumetric indices in normal older controls and individuals in the AD continuum.

Methods

Participants

The present study is a branch of a larger longitudinal study on cognitive impairment in the Brazilian population conducted in the D'Or Institute of Research and Education and the Federal University of Rio de Janeiro since 2011. Eligible participants were non-institutionalized older adults (>55 years old), with 7 or more years of schooling, native Brazilian Portuguese speakers, presenting memory complaints. Exclusion criteria were as follows: current major depressive disorder (according to the 5th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—DSM-5)[5]; current delirium[5]; history of severe psychiatric disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, intellectual disability, bipolar disorder)[5]; history of substance-related disorders[5]; severe visual or auditory impairments that precluded neuropsychological testing; refusal to complete the neuropsychological and neuroimaging protocol; and contraindications to brain MRI (e.g., ferromagnetic intracranial aneurysm clips or cardiac pacemaker).

Procedures

Initially, participants were submitted to a clinical interview by a physician, followed by physical examination, to verify the eligibility for the study. Subsequently, a neuropsychologist administered the Brazilian versions of the following instruments: the Mini-Mental State Examination[19], the Rey-Auditory Verbal-Learning test (RAVLT)[20], the Logical Memory and the Visual Reproduction subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale[21]. Verbal item-memory evaluation encompassed scores in RAVLT A5, which correspond to the number of acquired information in the last trial of the learning phase of the test, and in RAVLT A7, which refer to the 30-minute delayed recall trial[20]. Moreover, raw values of recollected items in Logical Memory and Visual Reproduction tests were used in this analysis.

Behavioral and functional assessment questionnaires comprised: the Neuropsychiatric Inventory[22], the Geriatric Depression Scale[23] and Lawton-Brody Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale[24].

Participants underwent an image acquisition protocol in a 3T magnetic resonance scanner (Achieva, Philips Medical Systems) including an isotropic high-resolution 3D T1-weighted sequence (TR/TE 13/ 1.4 ms; matrix 256 x 256 mm; FOV 240 mm; slice thickness 1 mm; 140 slices). Trained radiologist and medical physicists blinded to all evaluations analyzed the images for potential exclusion criteria. Moreover, visual assessment of images for potential hippocampal atrophy was conducted[13].

Raw T1-weighted DICOM sequences were processed by the Neuroquant® software–version 2 package. The algorithm used in this procedure has been previously detailed[15] and it encompasses the following stages: (i) quality assessment of structural MRI data for artifacts; (ii) gradient and B1 field corrections; and (iii) automated segmentation of brain regions-of-interest (ROIs), based on the neuroanatomical label attributed to each voxel within the targeted structure by a probabilistic atlas. Output of this method includes volumetric data (in cubic centimeters and percentage of intracranial volume—ICV) and images with each segmental structure marked in a specific color. ICV-corrected volumes of the total cortical grey matter (CGM), hippocampi and inferior lateral ventricles (ILV) from each hemisphere were extracted. In addition, the Hippocampal Occupancy Score (HOC) was automatically calculated, using the following equation: (left hippocampal volume / left hippocampal volume + left inferior lateral ventricle volume) + (right hippocampal volume / right hippocampal volume + right inferior lateral ventricle volume)[25].

Diagnoses

Participants were classified as normal controls (NC), Mild Cognitive Impairment due to AD (MCI) and dementia due to AD (DAD) using the 2011 National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association criteria[26,27]. For this purpose, analyses of clinical and neuropsychological data, as well as a visual inspection of MRI, were conducted and integrated by the whole multidisciplinary team. Evidence of AD pathology was determined by the presence of hippocampal atrophy using MTA visual assessment[13].

Ethical standards

All the participants provided a written informed consent prior to the inclusion in the study. Capacity to provide consent to participation in the research was determined during the initial interview and the following principles were adopted: (i) persons with cognitive impairment or dementia were presumed to have the capacity to consent unless established otherwise; (ii) regardless of their cognitive status, the participants’ preferences regarding their inclusion on the research were guaranteed; and (iii) since no structured instrument for consent assessment is available for older Brazilian population, a qualitative assessment was conducted and decisions to include or not the volunteer were based on the clinician’s impression about the subjects’ understanding and reasoning capacities[28,29].

The project was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the D’Or Institute under the protocol no. 226/11. The principles of the Resolution n. 510/2016 of the Brazil’s National Health Council, which regulates research involving human beings in the country were followed. In addition, the authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Statistical analysis

Data was checked for parametric assumptions based on visual inspection of histograms and values of skewness and kurtosis <1.96[30]. Mean differences in continuous data were compared across diagnostic groups using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), for normally distributed variable, or Kruskal-Wallis test, for the other cases. Welch’s test was applied for normally distributed data that violated the assumption of homogeneity of variance. Dunnett’s T3 post-hoc test was used to allow adequate pairwise comparisons, considering that groups showed unequal and small sample sizes [31,32]. Alternatively, when significant group differences were detected in Kruskal-Wallis test, serial Mann-Whitney tests were conducted to detect pairwise distinct medians. Distribution of sex among groups were analyzed using Pearson’s Chi Square. To assess convergent validity within episodic memory domain, partial correlations among the three memory tests were tested, adjusting for age and schooling. In addition, correlations between memory tasks and brain volumes were investigated. Analyses were conducted for the whole sample and for subjects with MCI and DAD. Value of α was adjusted for multiple comparisons and was set at p< 0.005 for all correlations. Due to unequal sample sizes across groups, correlation analyses were conducted using the whole sample and a combination of MCI+DAD participants. In addition, minimum correlation coefficient was corrected for the sample sizes, for a power of 80%, as follows: for the whole sample, lowest sizeable correlation was established as r = 0.4 to 0.5; whereas for analysis restricted to the MCI+DAD, moderate correlations corresponded to 0.5<r<0.7[28]. Rule-of-thumb for interpreting size of correlation coefficients were adopted, as depicted in previous studies[33]. Correlations between RAVLT and HOC were plotted. For all calculations, the IBM Statistical Package for the Social Sciences v. 25 (IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.) was used.

Results

Descriptive analyses

A total of 83 subjects were included in the study and were classified into three groups: NC (n = 47), MCI (n = 27) and DAD (n = 9). Participants in the NC group were significantly younger and presented more years of schooling than MCI subjects (p<0.001 and p = 0.001, respectively). No significant differences regarding sex distribution across diagnostic categories was found (p = 0.33). Performances in the MMSE and in all memory tasks distinguished the three diagnostic groups (p<0.001 for all comparisons). Adjusting for intracranial volumes, bilateral hippocampal and ILV volumes, as well as HOC and right CGM, differentiated the three groups (p<0.001 for all analyses), whereas left CGM volumes were only significant when comparing controls with MCI and DAD (p<0.001). Table 1 depicts those results.

Tab. 1. Sociodemographic characteristics, mean scores in cognitive assessment and brain volumes, corrected for intracranial volumes of the sample.
Sociodemographic characteristics, mean scores in cognitive assessment and brain volumes, corrected for intracranial volumes of the sample.

Convergent validity of memory tests

For the whole sample, after controlling for age and schooling, scores in RAVLT A5 strongly correlated with RAVLT A7 (r = 0.80, p<0.001) and were moderately associated with performances in Logical Memory (r = 0.45, p<0.001) and Visual Reproduction test (r = 0.47, p<0.001). Similarly, performance in RAVLT A7 was moderately related to Logical Memory (r = 0.54, p<0.001) and Visual Reproduction tasks (r = 0.56, p<0.001), whereas relationships between scores in Logical Memory and Visual Reproduction tests were also moderate (r = 0.44, p<0.001). For MCI+DAD groups, strong correlation was found between RAVLT A5 and A7 (r = 0.79, p<0.001), while a moderate association was detected between Visual Memory and RAVLT A7 (r = 0.53, p = 0.001). Those data are displayed on Table 2.

Tab. 2. Partial correlations among scores in cognitive tests and brain volumes (corrected for intracranial volumes), controlling for age and schooling.
Partial correlations among scores in cognitive tests and brain volumes (corrected for intracranial volumes), controlling for age and schooling.

Clinical-anatomical correlates of memory tests

For the whole sample, significant moderate positive correlations were found between bilateral hippocampal volumes and HOC, and RAVLT indices (A5 and A7). Left ILV moderately and negatively correlated with RAVLT and Visual Reproduction tests, whereas right ILV showed negative moderate associations with RAVLT A7 and Visual Reproduction. Right CGM related with scores in Visual Reproduction task. As for MCI+DAD group, both RAVLT A5 and A7 were moderately associated with bilateral hippocampal volumes and HOC. Right hippocampal volume was also moderately correlated with performance in Visual Reproduction. Those data are summarized in Table 2.

Fig 1 illustrates partial correlations between RAVLT A5 and A7 and HOC for the whole sample (A and B) and MCI+DAD (C and D). Scores in RAVLT A5 and A7 predicted, respectively, 35–37% and 39–48% of variance in HOC volumes.

Fig. 1.
Partial correlations between RAVLT and HOC for the whole sample (A and B) and MCI+DAD (C and D).

Discussion

Performances in episodic memory tests using stimuli of different materials (verbal or visual) and displaying divergent designs (delayed recall for word lists, stories or figures) were, at most, moderately correlated in our sample, comprising controls and subjects with MCI and DAD. Likewise, scores in those tasks were also fairly associated with hippocampal and other related brain volumetric indices. Of all those measures, the RAVLT (both A5 and A7) showed the best criterion validity, exhibiting significant, yet also moderate, relationships with HOC and hippocampal volumes in subjects within the clinical spectrum of AD. RAVLT A7 predicted 39–48% of variance in HOC volumes in our sample. In contrast, delayed-recall for items of stories (assessed by Wechsler Memory Scale’s Logical Memory subtest) did not correlate with hippocampal volume measurements. Figure-learning abilities (Wechsler Memory Scale’s Visual Reproduction subtest) moderately correlated with right hippocampal volumes in MCI+AD subjects.

Given the heterogeneity of tasks designed to investigate episodic memory deficits, determining whether they could be applied interchangeably for the assessment of patients with suspected AD-related disorders, or rather, if they tap different aspects of this cognitive domain would opportunely provide guidance when defining neuropsychological protocols in clinical and research practices. Additionally, appraising the impact of distinct approaches over outcomes could be valuable for the interpretation of contrasting results across studies. In this regard, ours findings are in line with a meta-analysis addressing cognitive impairments in the AD spectrum, which indicated that discrepant effect-sizes in episodic memory performances could be explained by heterogeneities regarding neuropsychological instruments adopted in the studies[34]. Hence, considering that medial temporal atrophy has been depicted as an early biomarker of AD pathology according to a large multicenter longitudinal research[35], it could be speculated that variations on methods to assess episodic memory may have accounted to some extent for inconclusive clinical-anatomical relationships across studies.

Some other inferences regarding the definition and the neural correlates of the episodic memory construct could be drawn from our results. The lack of robust associations among cognitive tests in our study might suggest that, instead of a unitary entity, episodic memory might represent a broad overarching functional system, involving a set of distinct and loosely correlated factors. Although the number and the nature of those components remain undetermined, task- and material-specific double-dissociation frameworks have been proposed in the literature, including: verbal versus visuospatial memory[36], temporal versus spatial memory[37], recent versus remote autobiographical memory[38] and memory for content (“item memory”) versus memory for context (“source memory”)[39], among others. Those paradigms may recruit different brain circuits, such as connections of the medial temporal cortices, but also widely distributed tracts throughout the frontal, parietal and occipital lobes[36,37,39,40]. Consistently, studies assessing neurodegenerative disorders with divergent patterns of brain changes (AD versus Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration and AD versus Parkinson’s Disease) have yielded discrepant amnestic profiles[10,41].

From this perspective, it could be accepted that differential demands regarding other cognitive abilities may also influence performances in memory tasks. Encoding and retrieval strategies applied during tests might differ according to the presence of neurocognitive disorders or to the properties of the test itself. For instance, memory for paragraph-length data (stories), as evaluated in Logical Memory test, requires contextual comprehension and semantic organization of the material[10]. In those cases, linguistic skills, semantic memory and executive function may favor associative binding of information. Appropriately, increased activation of brain areas related to working memory, such as the cingulate and the left inferior and middle frontal gyri, was detected during recollection of semantically-associated words in healthy older subjects[42]. On the other hand, encoding unrelated items from a word-list, as in the RAVLT, usually imposes more difficulty for engaging learning strategies. Hence, delayed-recall of items in word-lists has been depicted as highly dependent on the hippocampus[43], although some encoding processes have been described in the literature during this task, comprising mental imagery-creation or semantic link-inducing (for example, creating a narrative out of the words) [44,45], In addition, learning non-verbal material (as in Visual Reproduction test, for example) may also benefit from verbally recoding the stimuli[46].

Accordingly, data from a meta-analysis suggested that delayed-recall on word-lists may show higher accuracy for the diagnoses of MCI and DAD than impairments in story-learning tests[4]. It could be hypothesized whether higher demands of hippocampal-dependent processes in tasks assessing recollection of randomly unrelated set of items, as in word-lists, could account for this finding in AD subjects[47]. In contrast, recalling structured information in a story could be facilitated by relatively spared executive and language-related networks during the initial stages of the disorder. Furthermore, the relationship between scores in Visual Reproduction and the right hippocampal volumes may evoke a long-existing theory of left-right dissociation of memory systems. This disputed hypothesis implies that verbal information may depend on the left hippocampus, whereas visuospatial data may be stored within the structure in the right hemisphere[48,49].

Some limitations of the present study ought to be acknowledged. For example, since Neuroquant® does not provide segmentation of prefrontal cortex, relationships among memory indices and areas associated with fronto-executive functions could not be investigated[15]. Secondly, analyses were not controlled for medication use (e.g., antidepressants, antipsychotics and anticonvulsants), which could have negatively influenced cognitive performances. In addition, language and executive function were not analyzed in this study and effects of those abilities on episodic memory were merely inferential and should be considered with caution[50]. Moreover, the small sample size did not allow testing all the different stages of episodic memory within each task (acquisition and retention) without compromising the statistical power.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we state that different tests assessing episodic memory are not robustly correlated and should not be used interchangeably. Furthermore, performances in the RAVLT A7 significantly predicted up to 48% of the variance of the HOC volume in controls and individuals within the AD spectrum, whereas no other memory test showed similar associations with anatomical variables. Determining cognitive parameters mostly correlated with AD biomarkers might contribute for improving the characterization of the condition in clinical and research practices.


Zdroje

1. Allen TA, Fortin NJ. The evolution of episodic memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2013;110: 10379–10386. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301199110 23754432

2. Barnes J, Bartlett JW, van de Pol LA, Loy CT, Scahill RI, Frost C, et al. A meta-analysis of hippocampal atrophy rates in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30: 1711–1723. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.01.010 18346820

3. Jack CR, Bennett DA, Blennow K, Carrillo MC, Dunn B, Haeberlein SB, et al. NIA-AA Research Framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14: 535–562. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018 29653606

4. Weissberger GH, Strong JV, Stefanidis KB, Summers MJ, Bondi MW, Stricker NH. Diagnostic Accuracy of Memory Measures in Alzheimer’s Dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment: a Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Neuropsychol Rev. 2017;27: 354–388. doi: 10.1007/s11065-017-9360-6 28940127

5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 2013.

6. Morey RA, Petty CM, Xu Y, Pannu Hayes J, Wagner HR, Lewis DV, et al. A comparison of automated segmentation and manual tracing for quantifying hippocampal and amygdala volumes. NeuroImage. 2009;45: 855–866. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.033 19162198

7. Liu Y, Paajanen T, Zhang Y, Westman E, Wahlund L-O, Simmons A, et al. Analysis of regional MRI volumes and thicknesses as predictors of conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31: 1375–1385. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.01.022 20447732

8. Rugg MD, Vilberg KL, Mattson JT, Yu SS, Johnson JD, Suzuki M. Item memory, context memory and the hippocampus: fMRI evidence. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50: 3070–3079. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.004 22732490

9. Baek MJ, Kim HJ, Kim S. Comparison between the Story Recall Test and the Word-List Learning Test in Korean patients with mild cognitive impairment and early stage of Alzheimer’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2012;34: 396–404. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.645020 22263656

10. Zahodne LB, Bowers D, Price CC, Bauer RM, Nisenzon A, Foote KD, et al. The Case for Testing Memory With Both Stories and Word Lists Prior to DBS Surgery for Parkinson’s Disease. Clin Neuropsychol. 2011;25: 348–358. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2011.562869 21491347

11. Bayram E, Caldwell JZK, Banks SJ. Current understanding of magnetic resonance imaging biomarkers and memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement Transl Res Clin Interv. 2018;4: 395–413. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2018.04.007 30229130

12. Min J, Moon W-J, Jeon JY, Choi JW, Moon Y-S, Han S-H. Diagnostic Efficacy of Structural MRI in Patients With Mild-to-Moderate Alzheimer Disease: Automated Volumetric Assessment Versus Visual Assessment. Am J Roentgenol. 2017;208: 617–623. doi: 10.2214/AJR.16.16894 28075620

13. Menendez-Gonzalez M, Lopz-Muniz A, Vega JA, Salas-Pacheco JM, Arias-CarriÃ3n O. MTA index: a simple 2D-method for assessing atrophy of the medial temporal lobe using clinically available neuroimaging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2014;6. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00006

14. Stelmokas J, Yassay L, Giordani B, Dodge HH, Dinov ID, Bhaumik A, et al. Translational MRI Volumetry with NeuroQuant: Effects of Version and Normative Data on Relationships with Memory Performance in Healthy Older Adults and Patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment. Han D, editor. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;60: 1499–1510. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170306 29060939

15. Brewer JB, Magda S, Airriess C, Smith ME. Fully-Automated Quantification of Regional Brain Volumes for Improved Detection of Focal Atrophy in Alzheimer Disease. Am J Neuroradiol. 2009;30: 578–580. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A1402 19112065

16. Ochs AL, Ross DE, Zannoni MD, Abildskov TJ, Bigler ED, For the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Comparison of Automated Brain Volume Measures obtained with NeuroQuant® and FreeSurfer: Automated Brain Volume Measures. J Neuroimaging. 2015;25: 721–727. doi: 10.1111/jon.12229 25727700

17. Luo W, Airriess C, Albright J. The NeuroQuant Normative Database. Comparing Individual Brain Structures. [Internet]. CorTechs Labs—White Paper; 2015. Available: https://www.cortechslabs.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/10/Normative-Database-White-Paper.pdf

18. Ottoy J, Niemantsverdriet E, Verhaeghe J, De Roeck E, Struyfs H, Somers C, et al. Association of short-term cognitive decline and MCI-to-AD dementia conversion with CSF, MRI, amyloid- and 18F-FDG-PET imaging. NeuroImage Clin. 2019;22: 101771. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101771 30927601

19. Brucki SMD, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bertolucci PHF, Okamoto IH. Sugestões para o uso do mini-exame do estado mental no Brasil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2003;61: 777–781. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500014 14595482

20. Paula JJ de, Melo LPC, Nicolato R, Moraes EN de, Bicalho MA, Hamdan AC, et al. Fidedignidade e validade de construto do Teste de Aprendizagem Auditivo-Verbal de Rey em idosos brasileiros. Arch Clin Psychiatry São Paulo. 2012;39: 19–23. doi: 10.1590/S0101-60832012000100004

21. Spedo CT. Adaptação transcultural e propriedades psicométricas do subteste Visual Reproduction (Reprodução Visual I e II) da Wechsler Memory Scale—Fourth Edition (WMS-IV), (Escalas de Memória de Wechsler) para uma população do Brasil. Faculdade de Medicina de Ribeirão Preto. 2012.

22. Camozzato AL, Kochhann R, Simeoni C, Konrath CA, Pedro Franz A, Carvalho A, et al. Reliability of the Brazilian Portuguese version of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) for patients with Alzheimer’s disease and their caregivers. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20. doi: 10.1017/S1041610207006254 18257965

23. Paradela EMP, Lourenço RA, Veras RP. Validação da escala de depressão geriátrica em um ambulatório geral. Rev Saúde Pública. 2005;39: 918–923. doi: 10.1590/s0034-89102005000600008 16341401

24. Santos RL dos Virtuoso Júnior JS. Confiabilidade da versão brasileira da Escala de Atividades Instrumentais da Vida Diária. Rev Bras Em Promoção Saúde. 2008; 290–296. doi: 10.5020/18061230.2008.p290

25. Labs CorTechs. HOC: A predictor of disease progression from MCI to Alzheimer’s disease [Internet]. 2015. Available: https://www.cortechslabs.com/hoc/

26. Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman HH, Fox NC, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7: 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008 21514249

27. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7: 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005 21514250

28. Thorogood A, Mäki-Petäjä-Leinonen A, Brodaty H, Dalpé G, Gastmans C, Gauthier S, et al. Consent recommendations for research and international data sharing involving persons with dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14: 1334–1343. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.05.011 30293575

29. Palmer BW, Harmell AL, Pinto LL, Dunn LB, Kim SYH, Golshan S, et al. Determinants of Capacity to Consent to Research on Alzheimer’s Disease. Clin Gerontol. 2017;40: 24–34. doi: 10.1080/07317115.2016.1197352 28154452

30. Ghasemi A, Zahediasl S. Normality Tests for Statistical Analysis: A Guide for Non-Statisticians. Int J Endocrinol Metab. 2012;10: 486–489. doi: 10.5812/ijem.3505 23843808

31. Dunnett CW. Pairwise Multiple Comparisons in the Unequal Variance Case. J Am Stat Assoc. 1980;75: 796–800. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1980.10477552

32. Lee S, Lee DK. What is the proper way to apply the multiple comparison test? Korean J Anesthesiol. 2018;71: 353–360. doi: 10.4097/kja.d.18.00242 30157585

33. Schober P, Boer C, Schwarte LA. Correlation Coefficients: Appropriate Use and Interpretation. Anesth Analg. 2018;126: 1763–1768. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000002864 29481436

34. Bäckman L, Jones S, Berger A-K, Laukka EJ, Small BJ. Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychology. 2005;19: 520–531. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.520 16060827

35. Weiner MW, Veitch DP, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Cairns NJ, Green RC, et al. The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative: A review of papers published since its inception. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9: e111–e194. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.05.1769 23932184

36. Ezzati A, Katz MJ, Zammit AR, Lipton ML, Zimmerman ME, Sliwinski MJ, et al. Differential association of left and right hippocampal volumes with verbal episodic and spatial memory in older adults. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93: 380–385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.016 27542320

37. Ekstrom AD, Bookheimer SY. Spatial and temporal episodic memory retrieval recruit dissociable functional networks in the human brain. Learn Amp Mem. 2007;14: 645–654. doi: 10.1101/lm.575107 17893237

38. Tomadesso C, Perrotin A, Mutlu J, Mézenge F, Landeau B, Egret S, et al. Brain structural, functional, and cognitive correlates of recent versus remote autobiographical memories in amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. NeuroImage Clin. 2015;8: 473–482. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.05.010 26106572

39. Glisky EL, Polster MR, Routhieaux BC. Double dissociation between item and source memory. Neuropsychology. 1995;9: 229–235. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.9.2.229

40. Passaro AD, Elmore LC, Ellmore TM, Leising KJ, Papanicolaou AC, Wright AA. Explorations of object and location memory using fMRI. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00007

41. Pasquier F, Grymonprez L, Lebert F, Van der Linden M. Memory Impairment Differs in Frontotemporal Dementia and Alzhemier’s Disease. Neurocase. 2001;7: 161–171. doi: 10.1093/neucas/7.2.161 11320163

42. Miotto EC, Balardin JB, Savage CR, Martin M da GM, Batistuzzo MC, Amaro Junior E, et al. Brain regions supporting verbal memory improvement in healthy older subjects. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2014;72: 663–670. doi: 10.1590/0004-282x20140120 25252229

43. Wolk DA, Dickerson BC. Fractionating verbal episodic memory in Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage. 2011;54: 1530–1539. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.005 20832485

44. Cheke LG, Clayton NS. Do different tests of episodic memory produce consistent results in human adults? Learn Mem. 2013;20: 491–498. doi: 10.1101/lm.030502.113 23955172

45. Shing YL, Werkle-Bergner M, Li S-C, Lindenberger U. Associative and strategic components of episodic memory: A life-span dissociation. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2008;137: 495–513. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.3.495 18729712

46. Sperling G. Successive approximations to a model for short term memory. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1967;27: 285–292. 6062221

47. Barker GRI, Banks PJ, Scott H, Ralph GS, Mitrophanous KA, Wong L-F, et al. Separate elements of episodic memory subserved by distinct hippocampal–prefrontal connections. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20: 242. doi: 10.1038/nn.4472 28067902

48. Gleissner U, Helmstaedter C, Elger CE. Right hippocampal contribution to visual memory: a presurgical and postsurgical study in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1998;65: 665–669. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.65.5.665 9810934

49. Papanicolaou AC. The Hippocampus and Memory of Verbal and Pictorial Material. Learn Mem. 2002;9: 99–104. doi: 10.1101/lm.44302 12074997

50. Royall DR, Lauterbach EC, Cummings JL, Reeve A, Rummans TA, Kaufer DI, et al. Executive Control Function: A Review of Its Promise and Challenges for Clinical Research. A Report From the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2002;14: 377–405. doi: 10.1176/jnp.14.4.377 12426407


Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 10
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova

Svět praktické medicíny 1/2024 (znalostní test z časopisu)
nový kurz

Koncepce osteologické péče pro gynekology a praktické lékaře
Autoři: MUDr. František Šenk

Sekvenční léčba schizofrenie
Autoři: MUDr. Jana Hořínková

Hypertenze a hypercholesterolémie – synergický efekt léčby
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Hana Rosolová, DrSc.

Význam metforminu pro „udržitelnou“ terapii diabetu
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Milan Kvapil, CSc., MBA

Všechny kurzy
Kurzy Podcasty Doporučená témata Časopisy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#