#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

A multidimensional understanding of prosperity and well-being at country level: Data-driven explorations


Authors: Mohsen Joshanloo aff001;  Veljko Jovanović aff002;  Tim Taylor aff003
Authors place of work: Department of Psychology, Keimyung University, Daegu, South Korea aff001;  Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Novi Sad, Novi Sad, Serbia aff002;  Interdisciplinary Ethics Applied Centre, University of Leeds, Holmfirth, England, United Kingdom aff003
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 14(10)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223221

Summary

Social scientists have been interested in measuring the prosperity, well-being, and quality of life of nations, which has resulted in a multiplicity of country-level indicators. However, little is known about the factor structure of these indicators. We explored the structure of quality of life, using country-level data on tens of subjective and objective indicators. Applying factor analysis, we identified three distinct factors that exhibited both overlap and complementarity. This structure was replicated in data from previous years and with a partially different set of variables. The first factor, ‘socio-economic progress’, is dominated by socio-political and economic indicators but also includes life satisfaction, which thus appears to reflect objective living conditions. The second factor, ‘psycho-social functioning’, consists of subjective indicators, such as eudaimonic well-being and positive affective states. The third, ‘negative affectivity’, comprises negatively-valenced affective states. The three macro-factors of societal quality of life demonstrated moderate intercorrelations and differential associations with cultural and ecological variables, providing support for their discriminant validity. Finally, country and regional rankings based on the three societal factors revealed a complex picture that cautions against over-reliance on any single indicator such as life satisfaction. The results underline the need for a broadly-based approach to the measurement of societal quality of life, and provide an empirically-derived multidimensional framework for conceptualizing and measuring quality of life and well-being at country level. This study is thus an initial empirical step towards systematizing the multiple approaches to societal quality of life.

Keywords:

Quality of life – Mental health and psychiatry – Health education and awareness – Behavioral and social aspects of health – Factor analysis – suicide – Global health – Happiness

Introduction

Well-being is an elusive construct, which defies easy and unambiguous definition. Understanding the nature and structure of well-being has been one of the challenging tasks in social science research and philosophy. At the philosophical level, there are several rival theories which make competing claims about what well-being consists in [1]. In empirical research too, a number of operationalizations, conceptualizations, and definitions of the well-being construct have been proposed across disciplines [2, 3]. We are well past the era when traditional income-based economic indicators were considered sufficient indicators of societal well-being. Humanistic and social measures (e.g., health, education, and social capital) are increasingly emphasized and incorporated in empirical research on well-being [4]. This multiplicity of theory may lead to a major challenge by hampering consensus in conceptualizing well-being both at the individual and societal levels. Yet, this multiplicity also provides a diverse dashboard of operationalizations and approaches to enable a comprehensive understanding of the complex construct of well-being while avoiding reductive and simplistic approaches [5].

We take an empirically oriented and pluralistic approach to provide an integrative and multidimensional framework for conceptualizing and appraising quality of life, prosperity, and well-being at societal level. In doing so, we bring together a relatively comprehensive array of indicators of societal quality of life which has never before been collectively investigated in a single study. Our wide-ranging suite of indicators covers subjective and objective, as well as hedonic and eudaimonic aspects of quality of life. Our results suggest that the variables group into overlapping and supplementary dimensions. We believe this empirically-based categorization of the indicators has the potential to add new depth and insight to our understanding of prosperity and well-being at the societal level.

Measurement of societal well-being

Gaining insight into the nature of societal well-being (or quality of life, prosperity, and societal progress and development) has been inextricably linked with the question of how to measure this construct. The construct has traditionally been measured using economic indicators such as national income per capita and gross domestic product (GDP) per capita [6]. However, in recent years the inadequacy of economic indicators as proxies of well-being has been widely recognized and discussed (for an overview, see [7]). A number of non-economic dimensions of well-being had been systematically proposed, initially within the social indicators movement in the late 1960s and 1970s, to supplement economic measures as indicators of a country’s prosperity and progress [8].

Beyond the academic sphere, governments and international organizations too have increasingly recognized the need to go beyond economic measures. The UK, for example, instituted a programme entitled Measuring National Well-being, using a wide range of well-being measures, following the then Prime Minister, David Cameron’s 2010 declaration of intent to measure national progress ‘not merely by standard of living, but by quality of life’. Similarly, France has introduced quality of life measures in response to the findings of a Commission on the Measurement of Economic Performance and Social Progress [9]. Other developed countries, and international bodies such as the European Union (‘GDP and Beyond’) and the OECD (the Better Life Index) have introduced similar initiatives [10].

The predominant view in modern well-being research is that societal well-being is essentially a complex, multidimensional construct, including both objective and subjective indicators, and accordingly a comprehensive assessment of well-being must also capture both these aspects [11, 12]. The objective approach, originating in economics, focuses on material resources and objective indicators as proxies for well-being, whereas the subjective approach, originating from social survey research and psychology, concerns how individuals experience their lives and focuses on subjective evaluations of life and individuals’ inner feelings [13, 14]. The multidimensional approach to human well-being still needs to answer the question of which dimensions should be included in the conceptualization of well-being. This problem is especially evident regarding subjective indicators, due to a variety of well-being models developed in psychology over the past few decades. In general, well-being models in psychology take one (or both) of two relatively distinct, yet overlapping approaches to human well-being: the hedonic (or subjective) perspective and the eudaimonic perspective [1517], which are discussed below.

Hedonic/Subjective well-being

The hedonic perspective on well-being emphasizes the cognitive and affective evaluations of one’s life, and is usually operationalized as subjective well-being (SWB), which includes positive affect, negative affect, and overall life satisfaction [18]. Probably the most widely used and reported SWB indicator has been life satisfaction, which is increasingly being promoted as an alternative measure of well-being to inform public policy [19]. Consequently, most large-scale surveys of quality of life include a single-item (or longer) scale of life satisfaction as a key measure of well-being. Outside the realm of empirical social science, some philosophical theories define well-being in terms of life satisfaction [20]. Life satisfaction’s attractiveness to those who favor a simple metric of well-being can be explained by the fact that, even if it is not regarded as definitive of well-being in its own right, it can be seen as equivalent to a person’s own assessment of their well-being, taking into account other aspects [1]. Thus, while most individual measures can only plausibly capture part of the picture regarding well-being, life satisfaction has a reasonable claim to be an indicator of a person’s overall well-being.

Reliance on life-satisfaction as the sole measure of well-being rests on two assumptions: a) that a score on a life-satisfaction scale accurately reflects a person’s assessment of their own well-being; and b) that a person is a reliable judge of their own well-being. Both of these assumptions are open to question. There is evidence that life-satisfaction scores (and indeed, other SWB measures) adapt to circumstances, an effect first identified by Brickman, Coates, and Janoff-Bulman [21], and are susceptible to a variety of influences which are arguably unrelated to well-being, such as social comparisons [22] and ethical norms [23]. There is considerable debate about the extent to which these effects undermine subjective well-being measures, if at all [24, 25].

In measuring well-being some authors have exclusively focused on subjective indicators. For example, the Australian Unity Well-Being Index measures well-being by merely assessing satisfaction across several aspects of life [26]. SWB, particularly life satisfaction, has been advocated by some researchers as the gold-standard measure for assessing well-being [27]. However, many researchers have criticized this approach as arbitrary, simplistic, and reductive [5, 28]. Diener, Inglehart, and Tay [29] point out that “… life satisfaction measures have clear limits, and provide only one type of information to policymakers.” (pp. 521–522). In her review of societal well-being variables, Delle Fave [30] concludes that “there is no consensus on which subjective variables could be the ideal and most reliable indicators of well-being” (p. 85). In the present article, we take the view that, whilst life satisfaction remains a useful measure of well-being, it would be unwise to rely solely upon this single measure and that a broader mix of subjective and objective measures is more appropriate to measure national well-being and quality of life. In fact, there is increasing recognition of the need to integrate hedonic and eudaimonic approaches to well-being rather than relying solely on one and dismissing the other [15, 31].

Eudaimonic well-being

Eudaimonic well-being concerns psychological and social qualities that make a life worth living [32]. Most definitions of eudaimonic well-being exclude emotional components and instead focus on the fulfillment of human potential, psycho-social skills, a meaningful life, and self-actualization [33, 34]. Ryff’s [35] model of psychological well-being, which includes six key dimensions of autonomy, personal growth, self-acceptance, purpose in life, environmental mastery, and positive relationships, seems to be currently the most dominant in the field of eudaimonic well-being at the individual level of analysis. Other eudaimonic models posit highly similar qualities and skills [36].

The majority of previous global studies on well-being and discussions on the policy implications of well-being have focused on SWB indicators, especially life satisfaction [37]. Eudaimonic well-being measures have been rarely used in global well-being studies and largely ignored in debates on public policy. This seems to be largely due to the fact that comparable multinational data on eudaimonic well-being are only recently beginning to emerge. Huppert and So [31], provided a preliminary examination of eudaimonic well-being based on the data collected in the sixth round of the European Social Survey in 29 European countries. Joshanloo [33] developed a new index of eudaimonic well-being across 166 countries (capturing learning experience, social support, respect, efficacy beliefs, sense of freedom, and pro-sociality), which makes global studies of eudaimonic well-being possible. Individual-level studies suggest that hedonic and eudaimonic aspects are related, yet distinct components of mental well-being [16]. Joshanloo’s [33] results replicate this finding at the societal level. Thus, there is a strong case for including these both hedonic and eudaimonic aspects in holistic models of well-being since they capture different components of human well-being [38] and have differential relationships with other variables [39].

Measurement of societal well-being

Although most studies of well-being have focused on individual differences, the assessment of well-being at the country level has also flourished over the past few decades [40]. Although it is a widely researched topic in recent years, there is no consensus about the conceptualization and measurement of societal well-being. Different organizations and authors across various fields have proposed an array of dimensions and composite indicators in order to measure progress towards valuable goals and to enable informative and meaningful cross-country comparison [41].

One of the first attempts to measure material and social prosperity via multiple dimensions was made by the United Nations Development Programme (UNDP), when it introduced the Human Development Index (HDI) in 1990. The HDI is probably the best known composite well-being index, capturing three dimensions: longevity, education, and income. However, the HDI has received some criticisms (for an overview, see [42]); the main criticism being its narrow definition of well-being and the inclusion of too few dimensions. Thus, a number of alternative approaches to the measurement of prosperity and well-being at the national level have been developed in recent years, in order to capture additional relevant aspects of human well-being not included in the HDI. For example, in an effort to select and study quality of life variable in a systematic way, Diener [43] developed a value-based index of societal quality of life, where variables selected for measuring quality of life are reflective of prominent values endorsed across societies. This value-based index of quality of life is grounded on the universal structure of values constructed by Schwartz [44], with two proxy variables measuring each of the seven universal values of hierarchy, mastery, affective autonomy, intellectual autonomy, egalitarian commitment, harmony, and conservatism.

The Legatum Prosperity Index, first launched in 2007, measures human progress by assessing nine dimensions: Economic Quality, Business Environment, Governance, Personal Freedom, Social Capital, Safety and Security, Education, Health, and the Natural Environment (for a detailed description of the dimensions, see [45, 46]). The HDI and Legatum Prosperity Index are closely related and reliable measures [47], but although they share approximately 75% of their variance, it has been argued that the Legatum Prosperity Index is a more holistic and comprehensive measure than the HDI [45]. Other examples include the Better Life Index [9, 48] and the Social Progress Index (SPI; [49]), which are available for a smaller number of countries.

There are many other models and measures of national well-being (as reviewed, for example, in [50]). Despite notable differences in approaches to the measurement of societal well-being and prosperity, there are also important similarities between different approaches. For example, Phillips [28] provides an integrative review of the societal quality of life models and identifies four common components: civic integration, equal status (e.g., reciprocity, trust, and tolerance), sustainability, and social cohesion.

The purpose of the present study and variable selection

Our interest is in how well a nation is doing in a broad range of domains that reflect various aspects of national prosperity and quality of life. An important limitation of previous studies on societal prosperity and well-being is a lack of attention to the factor structure of the indicators at the societal level, typically including a limited range of dimensions, and focusing on the individual level of analysis. By including a wide range of indicators, both subjective and objective, the present study is expected to provide a more comprehensive evaluation of the structure of prosperity at societal level than previous studies. Our primary aim was to reduce the large array of quality of life variables to a smaller number of derived variables. Thus, we factor analyzed the national indicators to identify the underlying factors or dimensions of societal quality of life.

As previously noted, there is a wide range of views about which dimensions should be included when measuring prosperity. It is clear that not all aspects can be included in a single study, so every approach to selecting the key dimensions has some limitations. Here, we draw extensively upon both personal and societal well-being research but our sole focus is upon the national, rather than the individual level. Thus, we include both objective (such as income inequality and suicide rates) and subjective (such as life satisfaction and eudaimonic well-being) indicators. Our subjective indicators are aggregations of countries’ subjective judgments (as reflected in global surveys). Many of the indicators we include (such as the prosperity indices) are calculated based on both subjective and objective data.

In addition to our commitment to include both subjective and objective measures, we used three other criteria for the selection of component variables. First, given the emphasis on the complementarity of hedonic and eudaimonic aspects, we intended to include indicators of both aspects of well-being. Second, we were committed to including not only positive well-being variables (e.g., life satisfaction, positive affect, and eudaimonic well-being) but also negative mental health variables (i.e., negative affect, depression, anxiety, and suicide). Grinde [51] argues that the mammalian brain has a positive mood as a default setting, so activation of negative feelings is likely to be the main cause of low well-being. Therefore, negative emotions and mental disorders are as important as positive indicators of well-being. Finally, we only included dimensions available in most countries of the world, to maximize the global coverage of the analysis.

Based on these criteria, an initial set of 24 indicators was developed, which are introduced in the methods section and listed in Table 1. Using the data from the Gallup World Poll (GWP), we included the SWB measures of life satisfaction (both present and future), positive affect (emotions: enjoyment, rest, smile/laughter), and negative affect (emotions: stress, sadness, worry, and anger). We also included the newly developed GWP-based measure of eudaimonic well-being [33]. To complement hedonic and eudaimonic well-being indices from the GWP, we also included: (a) eight dimensions of prosperity as measured by Legatum Institute [45, 46]: economic quality, business environment, governance, personal freedom, safety and security, education, health, and natural environment; (b) three proxy measures of mental health: the disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for depression and anxiety, two frequent mental disorders, and suicide rate; (d) income inequality measured through the Gini Coefficient; (e) globalization measured with the KOF Globalization Index, which assesses three dimensions of globalization: economic, social, and political; (f) friendship opportunities, as an important aspect of social capital and interpersonal relationships.

Tab. 1. Results of exploratory factor analysis (2015–2017, N = 128).
Results of exploratory factor analysis (2015–2017, N = 128).

We chose the Legatum Prosperity Index over other prosperity models due to its comprehensiveness (covering nine prosperity domains), its inclusion of both subjective and objective data, and its wide geographical (149 countries) and time (since 2007) coverage. However, we excluded the social capital index, given its large overlap with the eudaimonic well-being index. Yet, we included as a separate variable a GWP item used in the social capital index to capture friendship opportunities. We also included measures of income inequality and globalization, which are two other important indicators capturing societies’ functioning [5254]. It is important to point out that our assortment of variables cover all the important aspects of societal quality of life emphasized in Phillips’s [28] and others’ integrative reviews of the literature. For example, whereas many models of societal prosperity and well-being have ignored environmental sustainability, we measure sustainability via one of the sub-indices of the Legatum Prosperity Index. The natural environment sub-index measures the quality of the natural environment, environmental pressures, and preservation efforts [45, 46].

In addition to examining the structure of prosperity at the national level, the present study also included a number of external variables to establish the nomological networks of the emerging prosperity factors, and examine their discriminant validity. There is ample evidence to suggest that various indicators of well-being have differential associations with other variables. For example, Helliwell, Huang, Wang, and Shiplett [55] found that whereas per-capita income and healthy life expectancy had significant effects only on life satisfaction, freedom and generosity had larger influences on positive affect than on life satisfaction. Diener, Ng, Harter, and Arora [56] found that economic factors (e.g., income, luxury conveniences) were consistently better predictors of life satisfaction than of affective well-being, whereas socio-psychological factors (e.g., fulfillment of psychological needs) were better predictors or affective well-being than life satisfaction. Corruption at the national level has been also found to be more strongly associated with life satisfaction than with positive and negative affect [57]. Therefore, we expected the emerging factors to exhibit differential relationships with external variables. We expected this analysis, not only to establish the discriminant validity of the emerging dimensions, but also to provide additional insights into the nature of these dimensions.

Drawing on prior research, we included a wide range of factors that have been found to affect national well-being, including socio-economic [58], cultural [59], demographic [60] and ecological [61] factors, namely, Hofstede’s dimensions of national culture [62], Inglehart–Welzel cultural dimensions [63], religiosity [64], urbanization [65], national age [66], and indices of thermal climate [67]. In selecting the external variables, we were guided by recent major reviews of the literature [68, 69].

In sum, we aimed at exploring the factor structure of the 24 prosperity and well-being indicators at the societal level and to establish the nomological network of the emerging factors. The general predictions of the present study were: (1) that prosperity will have a multidimensional structure at the national level, and the indicators would form distinct factors, and (2) that different dimensions would show interpretable and differential associations with the external cultural, demographic, and ecological variables.

Methods

Participants

The Gallup World Poll (GWP) dataset between 2015 and 2017 was used to construct the subjective country-level indices. Using randomly selected and nationally representative samples, GWP continually surveys residents in a large number of countries. The final sample of the study consisted of 457,129 individuals in 153 countries for whom data were available on the variables of the study. The average age in the whole sample was 41.72 (SD = 17.77, min = 15, max = 99). Women constituted 53.4% of the sample. The names of the countries, national sample sizes, gender ratios, and average age scores are reported in the supplementary material (S1 Table).

Measures

Variables used in the factor analyses included: life satisfaction (ladder), future life satisfaction, enjoyment, worry, sadness, stress, anger, laughter/smile, eudaimonic well-being, friendship opportunities, rest, economic quality, business environment, governance, personal freedom, safety and security, education, health, natural environment, globalization, depression, anxiety, suicide, and income inequality. It is noteworthy that depression, anxiety, suicide, and income inequality were initially used as indicators in factor analysis, yet based on the initial results we decided to remove them from factor analysis and instead use them as external variables. In a replication analysis, we also included life satisfaction these days, happiness, and purpose in life based on items available in older Gallup data. Variables used as external variables to establish the nomological network of the emerging factors included power distance, uncertainty avoidance, individualism, masculinity, self-expression values, secular-rational values, national age, urbanization, heat demands, and cold demands.

Gallup-based variables included life satisfaction, future life satisfaction, enjoyment, worry, sadness, stress, anger, laughter/smile, friendship opportunities, rest, life satisfaction these days, happiness, purpose in life, and religiosity. All of the GWP variables are based on data collected between 2007–2012 and 2015–2017. We use 2015–2017 data for our main analyses, and 2007–2012 data for replicating the factor analytic findings. The study also included external variables (i.e., variables not from the GWP). All of the external variables refer to the year 2015 (or 2007 for the replication analysis), unless otherwise stated below.

Eudaimonic well-being

This national index [33] was used to measure optimal functioning. The scores are based on the GWP data and refer to the years 2015–2017 and 2007–2012. This index is composed of seven GWP items measuring learning experience, social support, respect, efficacy beliefs, sense of freedom, and pro-sociality (i.e., helping strangers and volunteering).

Other Gallup-based variables

These variables were each measured with a single item. The GWP items used for measuring these variables are shown in S2 Table in the supplementary material. The scores were averaged in each country to construct a national score for that country. Three variables of life satisfaction these days, happiness, and purpose in life were not available in the period 2015–2017, and hence were only used in the analysis based on the 2007–2012 data.

National prosperity

Eight out of nine sub-indices of the Legatum Prosperity Index related to 2007 and 2015 [45, 46] were used to measure various aspects of national prosperity. The nine “pillars” of prosperity include economic quality, business environment, governance, personal freedom, social capital, safety and security, education, health, and natural environment. Each of the sub-indices consists of dozens of objective and subjective variables. More information can be obtained from http://www.prosperity.com. As some of the items used to build the eudaimonic well-being index have also been used in developing the social capital index, social capital was excluded in this study (four out of seven items of the eudaimonic well-being index are shared with the Legatum social capital index, which we consider a large overlap).

It is also noteworthy that the efficacy item used in the eudaimonic well-being index is also used in the business environment subindex of the prosperity index. Some of the affect items (i.e., enjoyment, rest, smile/laughter, and sadness) have been used to construct the health subindex. However, the health subindex consists of a large number of other indicators, and more importantly in the factor analyses, these affect variables and health were found to load on separate factors. Finally, the freedom item which is used in the eudaimonic well-being index is also part of the personal freedom subindex. Again, the personal freedom subindex consists of a large number of other indicators, and eudaimonic well-being and personal freedom loaded on separate factors. Therefore, we decided not to leave out these crucial variables from the analyses to enable a holistic exploration of these important variables.

Globalization

The KOF globalization index [70] related to years 2007 and 2015 was used as a comprehensive measure of globalization, assessing the economic, social, and political dimensions of globalization.

Depression and anxiety

The 2015 data from the Global Burden of Disease Study [71] was used to measure the disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) due to anxiety and depression, including years of life lost from premature death and years lived with less than full health. [One DALY “can be thought of as one lost year of healthy life and the burden of disease as a measurement of the gap between the current health of a population and an ideal situation where everyone in the population lives into old age in full health” [72, p. 1579]. The DALY, is a combinatory metric “designed to quantitatively measure the impact of various diseases and conditions on the productivity and well-being of people through a combination of mortality and morbidity estimates” [73, p. 2].]

Suicide

The 2015 age-standardized suicide rate was used to measure the prevalence of suicide in each country (http://www.who.int/gho/en/).

Income inequality

The GINI estimates for countries were obtained from the UNU-WIDER, World Income Inequality Database (WIID3.4), which is a database of information on income inequality (https://www.wider.unu.edu). All GINI estimates available for each country between the years 2005–2015 were averaged to obtain an income inequality estimate for each nation.

Hofstede’s dimensions of national culture

Hofstede’s four dimensions include power distance (“the degree to which the less powerful members of a society accept and expect that power is distributed unequally”), uncertainty avoidance (“the degree to which the members of a society feel uncomfortable with uncertainty and ambiguity”), individualism (“a preference for a loosely-knit social framework in which individuals are expected to take care of only themselves and their immediate families”), and masculinity (“a preference in society for achievement, heroism, assertiveness, and material rewards for success”) [74]. The scores for each country were obtained from Hofstede, Hofstede, and Minkov [75].

Inglehart—Welzel cultural dimensions

Scores of self-expression/survival values (the extent to which people prioritize individual choice over survival needs) and traditional/secular-rational values (the extent to which people emphasize modern values over traditional values such as religion, absolute standards, and conventional family values) were based on the data from the World Values Survey, Wave 6 (2011–2014). For countries that did not participate in Wave 6, data from Wave 5 (2005–2008) was used [76].

National age

In order to measure national age, percentages of the population that are aged 65 and above in each country were used. The data were obtained from https://data.worldbank.org.

Religiosity

We used Joshanloo and Gebauer’s [77] Gallup-based index of religiosity. This index is calculated as the percentage of people per country who stated that religiosity is an important part of their daily lives.

Urbanization

In order to measure the national degree of urbanization, percentages of the urban population in each country in the year 2018 were used. The data were obtained from the world urbanization prospects at https://esa.un.org/unpd/wup/.

Temperature

Van de Vliert’s [78] indices of thermal climate were used to measure temperature. The hot and cold measures are based on winter and summer deviations from a biologically optimal point of reference (i.e., 22°C or about 72°F). Climatic demands “are operationalized across each country’s or region’s major cities, weighted for population size, as the sum of the absolute deviations from 22°C for the average lowest and highest temperatures in the coldest month and in the hottest month” [78, p. 470].

Results

The analyses started with the main dataset related to 2015–2017. A principal axis factoring with promax rotation (Kappa = 2) was run using the 24 variables (the correlation matrix for the 24 variables is shown in S3 Table in an excel file). Only 128 countries with complete data were included. Scree plot suggested a 3-factor solution. The factor loadings of this initial model are shown in Table 1. Four variables demonstrated unexpected loadings. Anxiety and depression had positive loadings on the first factor. Income inequality had a positive loading on the second factor. Finally, suicide did not have a substantial loading on any of the factors. We decided to omit these four variables from the factor analysis and instead use them as external variables. This decision was driven by the fact that our aim was to identify unambiguous interpretable factors. In addition, these results cast doubt on the suitability of these variables to measure societal well-being (as further discussed in the discussion section). Thus, to maintain more intuitive factors, these four variables were removed from the analysis and factor analysis was repeated with 138 countries that had complete data on the remaining 20 variables.

Again the Scree test suggested a three-factor solution. The results are shown in Table 2. The three factors collectively explained about 68.02% of the variances in the variables. The first factor is loaded by globalization, and all aspects of national prosperity as well as life satisfaction (i.e., the ladder question). Generally speaking, this factor represents globalization and modern (vs traditional) values, as well as financial and socio-political development, and hence can be labeled as “Socio-Economic Progress”.

Tab. 2. Results of exploratory factor analysis (2015–2017, N = 138).
Results of exploratory factor analysis (2015–2017, N = 138).

The second factor is dominated by psychological and subjective variables, including eudaimonic well-being and positive affective states. With variables such as smile/laughter, eudaimonic well-being, future life satisfaction, enjoyment, and friendship opportunities, this factor measures a vital and psychosocially rich lifestyle. Thus, this factor can be labeled as “Psycho-Social Functioning”. Finally, the last factor is comprised of subjective feelings of worry, sadness, stress, and anger, and thus the factor can be labeled as “Negative Affectivity”. Higher scores on the last factor would mean higher levels of Negative Affectivity.

Replication of factor structure in data from 2007–2012

The same variables were constructed from GWP data collected between 2007 and 2012. The prosperity and globalization variables were related to 2007. As can be seen in Table 3, the emerging factor structure was largely similar to the results with more recent data. The same factors emerged. The 2007–2012 data included three extra variables for a smaller number of countries: happiness, life satisfaction these days, and purpose in life. These three variables were included in a separate factor analysis with a smaller sample size (N = 88). As shown in Table 4, the same factor structure was replicated. Life satisfaction these days loaded onto Socio-Economic Progress, and happiness and purpose in life loaded on Psycho-Social Functioning.

Tab. 3. Results of exploratory factor analysis (2007–2012, N = 141).
Results of exploratory factor analysis (2007–2012, N = 141).
Tab. 4. Results of exploratory factor analysis (2007–2012, N = 88).
Results of exploratory factor analysis (2007–2012, N = 88).

Factor correlations and country rankings

The consecutive analyses are entirely based on 2015–2017 data. The 20 variables (used in Table 2) were all z-standardized (to give a fairly equal presence to each variable in the final composite variables) and averaged to form three higher-level variables of Socio-Economic Progress, Psycho-Social Functioning, and Negative Affectivity. The Socio-Economic Progress score was calculated for nations with a single missing value as well. As shown in Table 5, observed correlations are stronger than latent correlations. In general, however, the correlations are modest, suggesting that a country’s standing on one of the dimensions does not strongly predict its standing on other dimensions. S1, S2 and S3 Figs in the supplementary material show the pairwise relationships between the variables. A three-dimensional scatterplot is shown in Fig 1. Cronbach’s alphas for each of the three variables based on the standardized variables are reported in Table 5, indicating acceptable internal consistency for each of the variables. Table 6 presents national scores and rankings.

Tab. 5. Factor correlations and alphas.
Factor correlations and alphas.
Tab. 6. Country scores.
Country scores.
Three-dimensional scatterplot.
Fig. 1. Three-dimensional scatterplot.

Regional distribution of national prosperity and well-being

Fig 2 shows the regional levels of the three prosperity variables. The names of countries in each category are shown in S4 Table. In terms of Socio-Economic Progress, Northern America, Australia, and New Zealand have the highest scores, whereas Sub-Saharan Africa and South Asia have the lowest scores. On Psycho-Social Functioning, Northern America, Australia, and New Zealand have the highest scores; Commonwealth of Independent States and Middle East and North Africa have the lowest scores. Finally, on Negative Affectivity, Middle East and North Africa have the highest score, whereas Commonwealth of Independent States, Australia, and New Zealand and East Asia have the lowest scores. Note that a high score on Negative Affectivity is not a ‘good’ score–high levels of negative affect are undesirable. The top-ranked countries here are Iraq and Syria, both blighted by civil war in recent years.

Regional averages.
Fig. 2. Regional averages.

The nomological network of the three dimensions

Table 7 presents the correlations between the three prosperity variables and the external variables of the study. The correlations with the four variables removed from the factor analysis are also presented in the table. As can be seen, most of the variables have differential associations with the external variables. This suggests that the variables have partly unique nomological networks.

Tab. 7. Correlations between the well-being dimensions and external variables.
Correlations between the well-being dimensions and external variables.

Discussion

Based on the results of the factor analysis, we identified three interpretable factors suggesting that, within the complex picture presented by the wide range of available indicators, distinct concepts are discernible. Below, we elaborate on the nature of the three factors.

Factor 1: Socio-Economic progress

This factor is loaded by variables that mostly reflect external conditions of life (as opposed to subjective and psychological feelings), country-level situations, and the functioning of public, social, and political institutions. Globalization and all of the prosperity sub-indices loaded on this factor. The indicators of this factor are partially based on people’s opinions as expressed in large-scale surveys, with survey questions mostly capturing people’s opinions about the socio-economic situations around them. The variables loading on this factor are primarily dependent on economic growth. Surprisingly, life satisfaction (ladder of life) too loaded on this factor, as its only purely subjective indicator. Cantril’s ladder of life has been shown to be more closely associated with socio-economic indicators than standard single-item life satisfaction measures (e.g., All things considered, how satisfied are you with your life as a whole these days?) [50]. However, in our supplementary analysis, we found that satisfaction these days also loaded on Factor 1 (Table 4). Unlike other subjective indices (such as affect and eudaimonic well-being), national life satisfaction does not seem to reflect well how life is subjectively experienced on a day-to-day basis. Instead, it seems largely to reflect how people perceive, or are affected by, economic and socio-political developments in their nations. This finding is consistent with evidence that economic, political, and social stress at the national level (e.g., crisis and war) can lead to rather rapid declines in a nation’s level of life satisfaction [40, 79, 80].

Life satisfaction can be seen as an indicator of the extent to which people are getting what they want in life. Our results suggest that, when people’s satisfaction responses are aggregated to calculate national scores of satisfaction, they align well with countries’ objective levels of development. Previous research has also found strong correlations (e.g., > .7) between national life satisfaction and economic and prosperity indicators [33, 40, 81]. Thus, it can be surmised that life satisfaction questions (particularly ladder of life) encourage people to think of their lives in economic terms [82]. This raises the question whether the overlap between country-level life satisfaction and Socio-Economic Progress is sufficient to create redundancy such that the simultaneous use of socio-economic indicators and life satisfaction at the country level adds little benefit, or worse, causes issues of multicollinearity (e.g., when using these indicators simultaneously as predictors in regression models). This large overlap also suggests that the availability of reliable measures of socio-economic development for a country may reduce the need for relying on life satisfaction measures, if the outcome variable of interest is Socio-Economic Progress. Conversely, however, given its strong loading on the first factor, national life satisfaction could be considered as a valid and highly useful summary index of general socio-economic development in large groups (e.g., nations). This is particularly so at the community or group level (e.g., with ethnic and cultural groups within nations, socio-economic classes, and neighborhoods), where reliable group-level indicators may be nonexistent.

Factor 2: Psycho-Social functioning

This factor is more subjective than Factor 1, being loaded by positively-valenced mental states (smile/laughter, future life satisfaction, well-rested, and enjoyment) as well as eudaimonic well-being and friendship opportunities. In the additional analysis, we found that happiness (close to enjoyment) and purpose in life (close to eudaimonic well-being) also loaded on this factor. These variables have in common an emphasis on inner feelings, psychological and interpersonal resources and functioning in life rather than external living conditions. This factor also covers social domains of well-being including altruism and social capital. In sum, this factor reflects a version of well-being that emphasizes living a worthwhile, hopeful, and enjoyable life involving pleasant states of mind but also developing and exercising personal and social skills.

Our finding that life satisfaction loaded on Factor 1 but affective variables loaded on other factors resonates with previous research concerning differential results for life satisfaction and positive affect. Earlier studies have found that national happiness is not correlated with life-satisfaction [83], and that affective measures are more sensitive to circumstances that evoke positive and negative emotions and less so to socioeconomic status, economic conditions, income, and material variables [8487]. The fact that life satisfaction measures loaded on Factor 1 and happiness loaded on Factor 2 suggests that different conclusions can be drawn using different SWB measures, and that findings produced by one measure might not hold for other measures. Thus great caution is needed when comparing the results of studies using different measures of well-being [88].

An important finding is that the present and future life satisfaction loaded on different factors. Gallagher, Lopez, and Pressman [89] argue that future life satisfaction measures the expectation of positive future outcomes and thus is a face-valid measure of optimistic expectations for the future. At the individual level, future life satisfaction has been found to have a stronger correlation with optimism and hope than do present and past satisfaction [90, 91]. The present country-level results are in keeping with the previous results. Our results show that, compared with present life satisfaction, future life satisfaction is associated less strongly with current socio-economic conditions and more strongly with positive affect and eudaimonic well-being. These results suggest that, at the country level, future life satisfaction is more likely to measure positive emotions and attitudes such as hope and optimism than evaluations of objective living conditions. Given the dearth of country-level studies involving future life satisfaction, these explanations and suggestions need to be further investigated in future studies. Yet, our findings do clearly indicate that future and present life satisfaction are distinguishable in interpretable ways, which needs to be acknowledged in future research.

We found that eudaimonic well-being also did not load on socio-economic prosperity. This too is consistent with previous research [33, 92]. The present results and previous research [93] leads us to predict that eudaimonic well-being is less likely than life satisfaction to change rapidly in response to economic and political stress at the national level, a prediction to be tested empirically in future research.

Factor 3: Negative affectivity

Of the three, Factor 3 is the most homogeneous: sadness, worry, stress, and anger, are all negatively-valenced affective states, and can be broadly categorized as forms of displeasure. Hence, this factor captures individuals’ day-to-day experience of unpleasant affect, and thus can be considered a measure of ill-being. However, it can be transformed into a positive well-being measure if reverse-scored. Whereas the separation of Factor 1 from the other two factors is interpretable in view of the level of subjectivity of the indicators concerned, the distinction between Factors 2 and 3 is not based on the levels of subjectivity but on the valence of the subjective states. This distinction is consistent with the predominant view that positive and negative affect are largely independent of each other, rather than two sides of the same coin [94]. Grinde [51] argues that this distinction is particularly important in that positive and negative affect have differential associations with one’s overall level of functioning.

Helliwell et al. [55] showed that positive emotions were more strongly associated with life satisfaction than were negative emotions and that positive and negative affect had differential relationships with other variables (e.g. generosity and perceptions of corruption). Our results indicate that, at the country level, positive affect is more strongly associated with eudaimonic well-being, future satisfaction, happiness, friendship opportunities, and feeling well-rested than is negative affect. Our results converge with the previous results in suggesting that positive and negative affect capture two different aspects of country-level well-being and should be studied independently.

Nomological networks

The three factors showed clearly distinct nomological networks. Below, we highlight some of the findings related to each of the factors.

Factor 1: Socio-Economic progress

This dimension was correlated with all of the external variables, except suicide and uncertainty avoidance. The strongest correlations were with national age (i.e., % aged over 65), religiosity (negative), self-expressive values, and individualism. This pattern is consistent with the general expectations of socio-economic development. For example, developed countries tend to have older populations, and endorse more secular and individualistic values [95]. The factor has a positive correlation with cold demands and a negative association with heat demands. It is also correlated with anxiety and depression. People in richer countries are less likely to report that their lives have a meaning [92], which may contribute to higher levels of depression. Research has also found that richer countries are also more likely to have a higher prevalence of some health problems such as obesity, and alcohol use [9698]. Therefore, a high level of Socio-Economic Progress is not a panacea, and these countries still have challenges to face. It is not clear whether these challenges are products of higher Socio-Economic Progress or remnants of pre-existing problems that Socio-Economic Progress has not yet been able to solve; or whether rising levels of Socio-Economic Progress will resolve these issues, intensify them, or leave them unaffected.

Factor 2: Psycho-Social functioning

This factor had relatively weaker correlations with the external variables than the other two factors, which reflects its strongly subjective content. It has its strongest correlation with self-expressive values, other correlations being nonsignificant, weak, or moderate. Unlike the other two factors, it had no association with temperature. This pattern of associations suggests that societies with higher levels of Psycho-Social Functioning are more likely to emphasize subjective quality of life and self-expression over economic and physical security [95], and are open to situations that are novel, unknown, and different from usual [75]. We speculate that Psycho-Social Functioning’s orthogonality to suicide as reflected in the multidimensional plots is largely a result of its association with meaning in life. A strong endorsement of self-expressive values is one of the main commonalities between countries that are high both on Socio-Economic Progress and Psycho-Social Functioning. Finally, Psycho-Social Functioning has a weak but significant positive correlation with anxiety.

Psycho-Social Functioning’s weaker associations with secular/individualistic values as well as economic and political situations suggests that developing countries are more likely to score well on this type of well-being than on Socio-Economic Progress. This is reflected in the fact that some African countries which suffer from life dissatisfaction have better standings on eudaimonic well-being (e.g., Mali, Libya, Nigeria).

Factor 3: Negative affectivity

This variable has weaker associations with the external variables than Socio-Economic Progress. Its strongest correlation is with secular values (negative), religiosity (positive), and cold demands (negative). The pattern of correlations for this variable suggests that negative affect is higher in more collectivistic, hotter, less urbanized countries, countries with younger populations and higher levels of uncertainty avoidance, power distance, and income inequality. This suggests that negative affect is inversely associated with socio-economic development and the endorsement of progressive values.

Dimensionality of national quality of life

Our data-driven exploratory analysis provides a plausible framework for understanding and appraising quality of life at societal level, and supports the view that country-level quality of life is a multidimensional construct, not a holistic entity that can be measured adequately by a single measure. Our two-dimensional plots of variables do not provide evidence of centrality for any of the variables. Our results are consistent with Diener and Suh’s [13] conclusion that objective, and subjective indicators are complementary, each capturing different aspects of societal well-being, and thus should be used in parallel. Therefore, it is necessary for future research to study the three dimensions separately as well as in tandem.

Quality of life of a given country differs substantially depending on the measure used to operationalize it. For example, the use of objective measures of material wealth may produce different rankings from measures of social and psychological resources [56, 99]. Similar findings were observed in the present study. All the countries that ranked in the top ten on the Socio-Economic Progress dimension (Switzerland, Finland, Norway, New Zealand, Sweden, the Netherlands, Denmark, Canada, Germany, Australia) are affluent countries from Nordic, Anglo, and Western-European societies. On the other hand, the top 10 countries in the psychosocial functioning dimension are more diverse (Uzbekistan, Indonesia, Norway, United Arab Emirates, Denmark, Canada, the Netherlands, New Zealand, Costa Rica, Ireland), as are the 10 countries that rank lowest in Negative Affectivity (Uzbekistan, Taiwan, Russia, Kazakhstan, Somalia, Kyrgyzstan, Mongolia, Azerbaijan, Singapore, Belarus).

Our regional analysis also shows the importance of considering the dimensions separately. Whilst the pattern of high and low scores across different regions was broadly similar between Socio-Economic Progress and Psycho-Social Functioning, South and Southeast Asia, Latin America and Sub-Saharan Africa scored higher on the latter than the former, whereas the reverse was true of European, Commonwealth of Independent States and Middle Eastern countries. Comparing Negative Affectivity with the other two factors revealed some more marked differences. Commonwealth of Independent States and East Asian countries had much better scores on this factor than the other two (a low score on Negative Affect being a good score); Northern America rather worse. These differences underline the complexity of prosperity and well-being at the national level. The fact that certain countries scored better or worse on Socio-Economic Progress than the other factors suggests that the aspects of prosperity which make up this factor do not tell the whole story of what influences well-being at the national level. Some countries, such as Nigeria and Paraguay, scored markedly better on Factors 2 and 3 than Factor 1 (Table 6). Therefore, a nation’s standing on one dimension may not accurately reveal its standing on the others. These findings militate against a reductive and simplistic approach that assumes a single gold standard measure of quality of life. Our results unmistakably indicate that using a single measure of national prosperity is inappropriate and may lead to simplistic conclusions.

Although positive and negative affect along with life satisfaction tend to form a SWB factor in studies on individual-level indicators of well-being (for a review, see [39]), in our national level analysis, these variables loaded on three distinct factors. Thus, conclusions about the structure of well-being obtained at the individual level should not be automatically translated to assumptions about the structure of prosperity and well-being at the national level.

An interesting finding was that Socio-Economic Progress and Psycho-Social Functioning had a stronger correlation than Psycho-Social Functioning and Negative Affectivity (Table 5). In general, the Negative Affectivity factor is more distinct from the other two factors. This finding is difficult to explain given that negative affect has been rarely studied at national level and not much is known about it. Yet, the finding is consistent with Helliwell et al.’s [55] findings suggesting that negative affect is less fully explained by other well-being and prosperity variables than are life satisfaction and positive affect, and is less strongly associated with life satisfaction than positive affect is. In sum, these results are suggestive of both complementarity of and overlap between the three dimensions.

Anxiety, depression, suicide, and income inequality

The four variables of anxiety, depression, suicide, and income inequality are considered by some as important components of country-level quality of life [28, 52]. For anxiety, depression, and suicide, these assumptions seem to be largely based on individual-level findings. For example, a depressed and anxious person is almost certainly not very happy. Yet, we found that, at the national level, these variables do not connect with the other indicators in an expected manner, which may cast doubt on their utility in measuring national quality of life (i.e., quality of life in the whole national population).

Depression had a positive loading on Factor 1, and its loading on Factor 3 was almost zero. Yet, it had a weak negative loading on Factor 2, which is consistent with the general expectations, since this factor involves variables such as meaning in life and positive affect that are antithetical to depression. Anxiety also had an expectedly positive loading on the first factor, and a loading in the expected direction but weaker on Factor 3. Given these mixed results, it is doubtful that depression and anxiety can be used as unambiguous indicators of national quality of life. Instead, we suggest using depression and anxiety as indicators of quality in life in depressed and anxious populations in a given country, as vulnerable populations that deserve special care. It is worth noting that we used the DALYs due to anxiety and depression (produced by the global burden of disease study), which are incidence-based measures that combine both mortality and morbidity into a single value [73]. Alternative national measures of anxiety and depression (e.g., prevalence-based measures) may lead to different conclusions.

Surprisingly, suicide had a weak but nontrivial negative loading on Factor 3, and trivial loadings on the other factors. Some previous studies have also found positive correlations between suicide rates and SWB [100, 101]. The suicide rate has been excluded from the most recent update of the Social Progress Index due to low correlations with other indicators of health and wellness [102]. Daly et al. [100] suggest that personal unhappiness may be at its worst when surrounded by those who are relatively more content with their lives. Diener and Suh [13] suggest that in high-SWB and high-income countries, there may be less social support and security when life goes badly as well as higher chances of loneliness which may lead to higher rates of suicide. In other words, higher levels of freedom in these countries have both the desirable effect of increasing SWB and the undesirable effect of increasing the possibility of suicide for people whose lives go badly. Based on these findings, national suicide rates cannot be used as an unambiguous nation-level quality of life indicator, and instead could be used for monitoring the status of people with suicidal tendencies and other affected populations in any given country.

Income inequality has shown positive, negative, and ambiguous associations with SWB [40, 103]. There is evidence that inequality may increase well-being in some nations but not others [104106]. Similarly, inequality’s association with economic growth is also not clear-cut [107]. We also found mixed results for income inequality. It had a negative loading on the first factor, and surprisingly a positive loading on the second factor. Therefore, our results dovetail with the previous evidence in suggesting that income inequality is not an unambiguous indicator of national quality of life. Therefore, we suggest not including income inequality as an indicator of country-level quality of life. Instead, income inequality could be studied in its own right as an important nation-level variable that can have complex and heterogeneous relationships with various aspect of national quality of life in various contexts and groups.

In view of the mixed pattern of associations with the other indicators, we excluded these variables from our factor analysis and instead used them as external variables to establish the nomological networks of the quality of life variables. We acknowledge that theory-driven treatments of national quality of life may be more open to including these four variables as component parts, yet, our data-driven approach hinders this on the grounds of the ambiguous factor analytic and correlational findings for these variables.

Relevance to public policy

This paper was intended to investigate holistically the structure of prosperity and well-being at the national level rather than to directly inform public policy. Nevertheless, we do believe that our findings would be of interest to policy makers. For example, the fact that our results support a broadly-based approach to measuring well-being, involving a mix of subjective and objective measures and avoiding over-reliance on any single type of measure is relevant to national well-being programmes as well as to academic research. In addition, the performance of nations varies across the three factors, so that a nation’s standing on one dimension does not reveal its standing on the others. This clearly cautions against over-reliance on unidimensional means of improving well-being–for example, an exclusive focus on economic prosperity. Thus, our results provide a comprehensive and multidimensional picture of well-being to inform the development of public policy. We hope that future studies will build on our work to provide results of more direct relevance to specific policy issues. For example, future studies might consider factor-analysis on sub-categories of measures organized by different policy objectives rather than across the full range.

Limitations and future research

Some of the limitations of this study need to be mentioned. First, the choice of indicators affects the results of factor analysis. The obtained factor structure in any factor analysis is inevitably contingent on the selected variables. Our selection of variables of national well-being is of course not definitive. Other subjective and objective indicators of national well-being could be incorporated in future studies on the structure of human well-being at the national level, to see if conclusions are affected in significant ways. As new scientific methods and data sources for measuring well-being at the nation level are emerging (for example, the use of language or text data to infer national well-being [108]), future studies can include more measures of well-being from diverse sources. Second, the GWP has its limitations, e.g., the single-item measures used to assess most of the concepts. Future research will need to use more reliable measures. Subjective measures of quality of life have been criticized on various grounds, including equivalence in translation and psychometrics [109, 110]. For example, researchers have emphasized that to make meaningful cross-cultural comparisons, equivalence and bias need to be addressed in cross-cultural surveys [111, 112]. More research in this area is needed to examine the magnitude of the effect of these artifacts on the measurement of well-being across cultures. Third, we used a limited number of external variables to validate the structure of national well-being that emerged in the present study. Future studies could include additional predictors or outcomes of well-being such as progressive taxation, personality traits, and crime rates (for a review, see [58]). Fourth, given the cross-sectional nature of our data, causality may not be inferred and future research needs to use longitudinal research designs to shed light on the direction of causality.

Finally, cross-national comparisons and ranking of countries are subject to a variety of caveats, which deserve special attention. The ranking of nations against the three factors: Factor 3 in particular–produced some surprising results. For example, whilst Iraq and Syria, which have endured bloody internal conflict, had the highest levels of negative affect, Somalia, a country experiencing similar strife, had one of the lowest. The predominance of former Soviet countries among those reporting the lowest levels of negative affect is also difficult to explain. We acknowledge that these–and perhaps other–results may have been influenced by other factors affecting the responses (e.g., for example, cultural factors or response sets) as well as by actual levels of negative affect. As argued by Tov and Au [113] cross-national differences in well-being and country ranking on well-being variables need to be interpreted with caution. The equivalence of meaning of SWB concepts (e.g., life satisfaction and happiness) should be carefully inspected in studies aimed at comparing countries on well-being measures, because these terms might mean different things to individuals from different cultures. For example, previous studies have found that meanings of, and attitudes to, terms such as “happiness” and “life satisfaction” may vary across languages and cultures [110, 114116]. Therefore, a necessary avenue for future research would be to tease out cultural influences systematically.

Concluding remarks

The limitations of GDP and similar wealth indicators have led to the development of numerous ideas and frameworks to measure national quality of life and provide information relevant to public policy. SWB, particularly its cognitive component (i.e., life satisfaction) has received extensive attention in this area of research and practice. Our results expanded these insights in various ways. For example, we found that, in comparison to life satisfaction, the affective components of SWB reflect information that is more likely to be different from that already captured by economic indicators. Moreover, our results on the structure of prosperity and well-being variables provide a systematic way to look at the diverse indicators and their interrelations. For example, the results suggest that eudaimonic well-being has stronger associations with positive affect than life satisfaction and Socio-Economic Progress, and eudaimonic well-being is also more likely than life satisfaction to provide information that is different from economic indicators. Our results also call into question the practice of categorizing affect and life satisfaction under the title of “subjective well-being” at the country level. This conceptualization stems from individual-level results and we showed in this article that it was not consistent with country-level results.

Delle Fave [30] highlights the current need for a more integrated and articulated perspective in the selection, use and interpretation of well-being indicators. One way to make progress towards satisfying this pressing need is data-driven explorations of the factor structure of the societal indicators of quality of life. Our study seems to be the first broad and inclusive study of this sort, aimed at organizing a large list of indicators in a conceptual structure. Inquiry into the structure of well-being at the national level is of crucial importance at this stage of development in this field of research. The results of such studies can help us avoid over-reductionism, and instead enable a systematic identification of elementary indictors to efficiently reduce the number of variables. Such studies help to balance the hegemony of particular approaches over others [117]. They can also support the development of empirically-informed conceptual frameworks. It is, however, appropriate to be cautious at this stage, in view of the fact that this study is among the first factor-analytic studies of prosperity and well-being at the country level. Thus, our findings should be considered preliminary. It is hoped that these results will stimulate more research endeavors aimed at clarifying the structure of national prosperity and well-being. It is noteworthy that studies of this nature involve ecological analyses the results of which do not necessarily have implications for individuals [83].

In conclusion, the results of our research paint a complex and promising–if provisional–picture of prosperity and well-being at the national level. The complexity of the relationships between the three well-being factors, and between them and other variables, underlines the need for a broadly-based approach to the measurement of well-being, avoiding over-reliance on any single indicator such as life satisfaction. We hope that other researchers will carry out further work to expand and advance these findings.

Supporting information

S1 Fig [docx]
The relationship between psycho-social functioning and socio-economic progress.

S2 Fig [docx]
The relationship between negative affectivity and socio-economic progress.

S3 Fig [docx]
The relationship between negative affectivity and psycho-social functioning.

S1 Table [docx]
Descriptive information for the main dataset (2015–2017).

S2 Table [docx]
Gallup items used in the study.

S3 Table [xlsx]
Intercorrelations between the 24 indicators.

S4 Table [docx]
Global regions (Gallup’s categorization).


Zdroje

1. Taylor TE. The markers of wellbeing: a basis for a theory-neutral approach. Int J Well Being. 2015;5: 75–90. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v5i2.5

2. Dodge R, Daly A, Huyton J, Sanders L. The challenge of defining wellbeing. Int J Well Being. 2012;2: 222–235. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v2i3.4

3. McGillivray M. Human well-being: issues, concepts and measures. In: McGillivray M, editor. Human well-being. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK; 2007. pp. 1–22.

4. Barrington-Leigh C, Escande A. Measuring progress and well-being: a comparative review of indicators. Soc Indic Res. 2018;135: 893–925. doi: 10.1007/s11205-016-1505-0

5. Jayawickreme E, Forgeard MJC, Seligman MEP. The engine of well-being. Rev Gen Psychol. 2012;16: 327–342. doi: 10.1037/a0027990

6. Bleys B. Beyond GDP: Classifying alternative measures for progress. Soc Indic Res. 2012;109: 355–376. doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-9906-6

7. Costanza R, Hart M, Posner S, Talberth J. Beyond GDP: The need for new measures of progress. The Pardee Papers, No. 4; 2009.

8. Land KC, Michalos AC. Fifty years after the social indicators movement: has the promise been fulfilled?: an assessment an agenda for the future. Soc Indic Res. 2018;135: 835–868. doi: 10.1007/s11205-017-1571-y

9. Stiglitz J, Sen A, Fitoussi JP. Report by the Commission on the measurement of economic performance and social progress; 2009. https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/documents/118025/118123/Fitoussi+Commission+report

10. Bache I, Scott K. Wellbeing in politics and policy. In: Bache I, Scott K, editors. The politics of wellbeing. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2018. pp. 1–22.

11. Gasper D. Subjective and objective well-being in relation to economic inputs: puzzles and responses. Rev Soc Econ. 2005;63: 177–206. doi: 10.1080/00346760500130309

12. McGregor JA, Coulthard S, Camfield L. Measuring what matters: The role of well-being methods in development policy and practice. ODI Development Progress Project Note, 4; 2015.

13. Diener E, Suh E. Measuring quality of life: Economic, social, and subjective indicators. Soc Indic Res. 1997;40: 189–216. doi: 10.1023/A:1006859511756

14. Western M, Tomaszewski W. Subjective wellbeing, objective wellbeing and inequality in Australia. PLoS ONE. 2016;11: e0163345. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163345 27695042

15. Henderson L, Knight T. Integrating the hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives to more comprehensively understand wellbeing and pathways to wellbeing. Int J Well Being. 2012;2: 196–221. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v2i3.3

16. Joshanloo M. Revisiting the empirical distinction between hedonic and eudaimonic aspects of well-being using exploratory structural equation modeling. J Happiness Stud. 2016;17: 2023–2036. doi: 10.1007/s10902-015-9683-z

17. Ryan RM, Deci EL. On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001;52: 141–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141 11148302

18. Diener E. Subjective well-being. Psychol Bull. 1984;95: 542–575. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542 6399758

19. Dolan P, Peasgood T. Measuring well-being for public policy: preferences or experiences? J Legal Stud. 2008;37: S5–S31. doi: 10.1086/595676

20. Sumner LW. Welfare, happiness, and ethics. Oxford University Press; 1999.

21. Brickman P, Coates D, Janoff-Bulman R. Lottery winners and accident victims: is happiness relative? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1978;36: 917–927. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.36.8.917 690806

22. Luttmer EFP. Neighbors as negatives: relative earnings and well-being. Q J Econ. 2005;120: 963–1002.

23. Haybron D. Life satisfaction, ethical reflection, and the science of happiness. J Happiness Stud. 2007;8: 99–138. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9006-5

24. Diener E, Lucas R, Schimmack U, Helliwell J. Well-being for public policy. Oxford University Press; 2009.

25. Taylor T. Adaptation and the measurement of well-being. Ethics Soc Welfare. 2014;8: 248–261. doi: 10.1080/17496535.2014.932417

26. Cummins RA, Eckersley R, Pallant J, van Vugt J, Misajon R. Developing a national index of subjective wellbeing: the Australian Unity Wellbeing Index. Soc Indic Res. 2003;64: 159–190. doi: 10.1023/A:1024704320683

27. Kashdan TB, Biswas-Diener R, King LA. Reconsidering happiness: the costs of distinguishing between hedonics and eudaimonia. J Posit Psychol. 2008;3: 219–233. doi: 10.1080/17439760802303044

28. Phillips D. Quality of life: concept, policy and practice. London; New York: Routledge; 2006.

29. Diener E, Inglehart R, Tay L. Theory and validity of life satisfaction scales. Soc Indic Res. 2013;112: 497–527. doi: 10.1007/s11205-012-0076-y

30. Delle Fave A. Subjective indicators of well-being: conceptual background and applications in social sciences. In: Bruni L, Porta PL, editors. Handbook of research methods and applications in happiness and quality of life. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing; 2016. pp. 71–89.

31. Huppert FA, So TTC. Flourishing across Europe: application of a new conceptual framework for defining well-being. Soc Indic Res. 2013;110: 837–861. doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-9966-7 23329863

32. Heintzelman SJ. Eudaimonia in the contemporary science of subjective well-being: psychological well-being, self-determination, and meaning in life. In: Diener E, Oishi S, Tay L, editors. Handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT: DEF Publishers; 2018.

33. Joshanloo M. Optimal human functioning around the world: a new index of eudaimonic well-being in 166 nations. Br J Psychol. 2018;109: 637–655. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12316 29846018

34. Lent RW. Toward a unifying theoretical and practical perspective on well-being and psychosocial adjustment. J Couns Psychol. 2004;51: 482–509. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.51.4.482

35. Ryff CD. Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;57: 1069–1081.

36. Huta V, Waterman AS. Eudaimonia and its distinction from hedonia: developing a classification and terminology for understanding conceptual and operational definitions. J Happiness Stud. 2014;15: 1425–1456. doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9485-0

37. Diener E, Seligman MEP. Beyond money: progress on an economy of well-being. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2018;13: 171–175. doi: 10.1177/1745691616689467 29592646

38. Keyes CLM, Annas J. Feeling good and functioning well: distinctive concepts in ancient philosophy and contemporary science. J Posit Psychol. 2009;4: 197–201. doi: 10.1080/17439760902844228

39. Joshanloo M. Structural and discriminant validity of the tripartite model of mental well-being: differential relationships with the big five traits. J Ment Health. 2019;28: 168–174. doi: 10.1080/09638237.2017.1370633 28868947

40. Diener E, Tay L. Subjective well-being and human welfare around the world as reflected in the Gallup World Poll. Int J Psychol. 2015;50: 135–149. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12136 25612012

41. Estes RJ, Sirgy MJ. Global advances in quality of life and well-being: past, present, and future. Soc Indic Res. 2019;141: 1137–1164. doi: 10.1007/s11205-018-1869-4

42. Dervis K, Klugman J. Measuring human progress: the contribution of the Human Development Index and related indices. Rev Econ Polit. 2011;121: 73–92. doi: 10.3917/redp.211.0073

43. Diener E. A value based index for measuring national quality of life. Soc Indic Res. 1995;36: 107–127. doi: 10.1007/BF01079721

44. Schwartz SH. Beyond individualism/collectivism: new cultural dimensions of values. In: Kim U, Triandis HC, Kâğitçibaşi Ç, Choi S-C, Yoon G, editors. Cross-cultural research and methodology series, Vol. 18. Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method, and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc; 1994. pp. 85–119.

45. Legatum Institute. Legatum Prosperity Index 2017: methodology report; 2017a. http://prosperitysite.s3-accelerate.amazonaws.com/9615/1186/6075/Legatum_Prosperity_Index_2017_Methodology_Report.pdf

46. Legatum Institute. The 2017 Legatum Prosperity Index; 2017b. https://prosperitysite.s3-accelerate.amazonaws.com/3515/1187/1128/Legatum_Prosperity_Index_2017.pdf

47. Otoiu A, Titan E, Dumitrescu R. Are the variables used in building composite indicators of well-being relevant? Validating composite indexes of well-being. Ecol Indic. 2014;46: 575–585. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolind.2014.07.019

48. Durand M. The OECD Better Life Initiative: How’s Life? and the measurement of well-being. Rev Income and Wealth. 2015;61: 4–17. doi: 10.1111/roiw.12156

49. Porter ME, Stern S, Green M. Social Progress Index 2015. Washington, DC: Social Progress Imperative; 2015.

50. Delhey J, Kroll C. A “happiness test” for the new measures of national well-being: how much better than GDP are they? In: Brockmann H, Delhey J, editors. Human happiness and the pursuit of maximization. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 2013. pp. 191–210.

51. Grinde B. Why negative feelings are important when assessing well-being. J Happiness Stud. 2016;17: 1741–1752. doi: 10.1007/s10902-015-9667-z

52. Hagerty MR, Cummins RA, Ferriss AL, Land K, Michalos AC, Peterson M, et al. Quality of life indexes for national policy: review and agenda for research. Soc Indic Res. 2001;55: 1–96. doi: 10.1023/A:1010811312332

53. Maggino F. Indicators of happiness vs. quality of happiness: methodology and theory. In: Bruni L, Porta PL, editors. Handbook of research methods and applications in happiness and quality of life. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing; 2016. pp. 196–228.

54. Nayyar D. Development through globalization? In: Mavrotas G, Shorrocks A, editors. Advancing development. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK; 2007. pp. 593–613. doi: 10.1057/9780230801462_31

55. Helliwell JF, Huang H, Wang S, Shiplett H. International migration and world happiness. In: Helliwell JF, Layard R, Sachs JD, editors. World happiness report 2018. New York: Sustainable Development Solutions Network; 2018. pp. 13–43.

56. Diener E, Ng W, Harter J, Arora R. Wealth and happiness across the world: material prosperity predicts life evaluation, whereas psychosocial prosperity predicts positive feeling. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010;99: 52–61. doi: 10.1037/a0018066 20565185

57. Tay L, Herian MN, Diener E. Detrimental effects of corruption and subjective well-being: whether, how, and when. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2014;5: 751–759. doi: 10.1177/1948550614528544

58. Jorm AF, Ryan SM. Cross-national and historical differences in subjective well-being. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43: 330–340. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt188 24585736

59. Steel P, Taras V, Uggerslev K, Bosco F. The happy culture: a theoretical, meta-analytic, and empirical review of the relationship between culture and wealth and subjective well-being. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2018;22: 128–169. doi: 10.1177/1088868317721372 28770649

60. Diener E, Tay L, Myers DG. The religion paradox: if religion makes people happy, why are so many dropping out? J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011;101: 1278–1290. doi: 10.1037/a0024402 21806304

61. Fischer R, Van de Vliert E. Does climate undermine subjective well-being? a 58-nation study. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2011;37: 1031–1041. doi: 10.1177/0146167211407075 21555504

62. Arrindell WA, Hatzichristou C, Wensink J, Rosenberg E, van Twillert B, Stedema J, et al. Dimensions of national culture as predictors of cross-national differences in subjective well-being. Pers Individ Dif. 1997;23: 37–53. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00023-8

63. Welzel C, Inglehart R. Agency, values, and well-being: a human development model. Soc Indic Res. 2010;97: 43–63. doi: 10.1007/s11205-009-9557-z 20390031

64. Joshanloo M. Cultural religiosity as the moderator of the relationship between affective experience and life satisfaction: a study in 147 countries. Emotion. 2019;19: 629–636. doi: 10.1037/emo0000469 29999383

65. Kim D. Cross-national pattern of happiness: do higher education and less urbanization degrade happiness? Appl Res Qual Life. 2018;13: 21–35. doi: 10.1007/s11482-017-9504-0

66. Bjørnskov C. Healthy and happy in Europe? On the association between happiness and life expectancy over time. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66: 1750–1759. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.01.031 18313190

67. Rehdanz K, Maddison D. Climate and happiness. Ecol Econ. 2005;52: 111–125. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2004.06.015

68. Diener E, Oishi S, Ryan KL. Universals and cultural differences in the causes and structure of happiness: a multilevel review. In: Keyes CLM, editor. Mental well-being. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 2013. pp. 153–176.

69. Veenhoven R. Subjective well-being in nations. In: Diener E, Oishi S, Tay L, editors. Handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT: DEF Publishers; 2018.

70. Gygli S, Haelg F, Potrafke N, Sturm J-E. The KOF Globalisation Index–revisited. Rev Int Organ. 2019;14: 543–574. doi: 10.1007/s11558-019-09344-2

71. Institute for Health Metrics and Evaluation. GBD compare data visualization. Seattle, WA: IHME, University of Washington; 2015. https://vizhub.healthdata.org/gbd-compare/

72. Strong K, Mathers C, Leeder S, Beaglehole R. Preventing chronic diseases: how many lives can we save? Lancet. 2005;366: 1578–1582. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67341-2 16257345

73. Parks R. The rise, critique and persistence of the DALY in global health. J Glob Health. 2014. http://www.ghjournal.org/the-rise-critique-and-persistence-of-the-daly-in-global-health/

74. Hofstede GH. National culture. 2018. https://www.hofstede-insights.com/models/national-culture

75. Hofstede GH, Hofstede GJ, Minkov M. Cultures and organizations: software of the mind. 3rd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2010.

76. Inglehart R, Haerpfer C, Moreno A, Welzel C, Kizilova K, Diez-Medrano J, et al. World values survey: round six—country-pooled datafile 2010–2014. Madrid: JD Systems Institute. 2014. http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSDocumentationWV.

77. Joshanloo M, Gebauer JG. Religiosity’s nomological network and temporal change: introducing an extensive country-level religiosity index based on Gallup World Poll data. Eur Psychol. Forthcoming 2019.

78. Van de Vliert E. Climato-economic habitats support patterns of human needs, stresses, and freedoms. Behav Brain Sci. 2013;36: 465–480. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12002828 23985270

79. Helliwell JF, Layard R, Sachs JD. World happiness report 2015. New York: Sustainable Development Solutions Network; 2015.

80. Tay L, Kuykendall L, Diener E. Satisfaction and happiness–the bright side of quality of life. In: Glatzer W, Camfield L, Møller V, Rojas M, editors. Global handbook of quality of life. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 2015. pp. 839–853.

81. Diener E. New findings and future directions for subjective well-being research. Am Psychol. 2012;67: 590–597. doi: 10.1037/a0029541 23163434

82. White SC. Introduction: the many faces of wellbeing. In: White SC, Blackmore C, editors. Cultures of wellbeing. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK; 2016. pp. 1–44.

83. Minkov M, Schachner M, Sanchez C, Morales O. A new worldwide measure of happiness explains national differences in suicide rates and cigarette consumption. Cross Cult Res. 2019;53: 355–384. doi: 10.1177/1069397118799688

84. Diener E, Kahneman D, Tov W, Arora R. Income’s association with judgments of life versus feelings. In: Diener E, Helliwell JF, Kahneman D, editors. International differences in well-being. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2010. pp. 3–15.

85. Inglehart R, Foa R, Peterson C, Welzel C. Development, freedom, and rising happiness: a global perspective (1981–2007). Perspect Psychol Sci. 2008;3: 264–285. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00078.x 26158947

86. Kahneman D, Deaton A. High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107: 16489–16493. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011492107 20823223

87. Kahneman D, Krueger AB, Schkade D, Schwarz N, Stone AA. Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science. 2006;312: 1908–1910. doi: 10.1126/science.1129688 16809528

88. Bjørnskov C. How comparable are the Gallup World Poll life satisfaction data? J Happiness Stud. 2010;11: 41–60. doi: 10.1007/s10902-008-9121-6

89. Gallagher MW, Lopez SJ, Pressman SD. Optimism is universal: exploring the presence and benefits of optimism in a representative sample of the world. J Pers. 2013;81: 429–440. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12026 23253230

90. Pavot W, Diener E, Suh E. The temporal satisfaction with life scale. J Pers Assess. 1998;70: 340–354. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa7002_11

91. Proyer RT, Gander F, Wyss T, Ruch W. The relation of character strengths to past, present, and future life satisfaction among German-speaking women. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2011;3: 370–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2011.01060.x

92. Oishi S, Diener E. Residents of poor nations have a greater sense of meaning in life than residents of wealthy nations. Psychol Sci. 2014;25: 422–430. doi: 10.1177/0956797613507286 24335603

93. Joshanloo M. Investigating the relationships between subjective well-being and psychological well-being over two decades. Emotion. 2019;19: 183–187. doi: 10.1037/emo0000414 29494203

94. Kuppens P, Ceulemans E, Timmerman ME, Diener E, Kim-Prieto C. Universal intracultural and intercultural dimensions of the recalled frequency of emotional experience. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2006;37: 491–515. doi: 10.1177/0022022106290474

95. Inglehart R, Welzel C. Modernization, cultural change, and democracy: the human development sequence. Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge University Press; 2005.

96. Griswold MG, Fullman N, Hawley C, Arian N, Zimsen SRM, Tymeson HD, et al. Alcohol use and burden for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet. 2018;392: 1015–1035. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31310-2 30146330

97. Joshanloo M, Jovanović V. Subjective health in relation to hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing: evidence from the Gallup World Poll. J Health Psychol. 2018. doi: 10.1177/1359105318820104 30582374

98. Morgen CS, Sørensen TIA. Global trends in the prevalence of overweight and obesity. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2014;10: 513–514. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2014.124 25048038

99. Rojas M. Happiness, research, and Latin America. In: Rojas M, editor. Handbook of happiness research in Latin America. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 2016. pp. 1–13.

100. Daly MC, Oswald AJ, Wilson D, Wu S. Dark contrasts: the paradox of high rates of suicide in happy places. J Econ Behav Organ. 2011;80: 435–442. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2011.04.007

101. Diener E. Subjective well-being in cross-cultural perspective. In: Grad H, Blanco A, Georgas J, editors. Key issues in cross-cultural psychology. Liese, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1996. pp. 319–330.

102. Scott S, Wares A, Epner T. Social Progress Index methodology report; 2018. https://www.socialprogress.org/assets/downloads/resources/2018/2018-Social-Progress-Index-Methodology.pdf

103. Ng W, Diener E. Affluence and subjective well-being: does income inequality moderate their associations? Appl Res Qual Life. 2019;14: 155–170. doi: 10.1007/s11482-017-9585-9

104. Reyes-García V, Angelsen A, Shively GE, Minkin D. Does income inequality influence subjective wellbeing? Evidence from 21 developing countries. J Happiness Stud. 2019;20: 1197–1215. doi: 10.1007/s10902-018-9992-0

105. Kelley J, Evans MDR. Societal inequality and individual subjective well-being: results from 68 societies and over 200,000 individuals, 1981–2008. Soc Sci Res. 2017;62: 1–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2016.04.020 28126092

106. Ngamaba KH, Panagioti M, Armitage CJ. Income inequality and subjective well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Qual Life Res. 2018;27: 577–596. doi: 10.1007/s11136-017-1719-x 29067589

107. Stiglitz JE. 8. Inequality and economic growth. Polit Q. 2015;86: 134–155. doi: 10.1111/1467-923X.12237

108. Hills T, Proto E, Sgroi D. Historical analysis of national subjective wellbeing using millions of digitized books. CESifo Working Paper; 2015.

109. Bond TN, Lang K. The sad truth about happiness scales. J Polit Econ. 2019;127: 1629–1640. doi: 10.1086/701679

110. Cummins RA. Measuring and interpreting subjective wellbeing in different cultural contexts: a review and way forward. 1st ed. Cambridge University Press; 2018.

111. Bieda A, Hirschfeld G, Schönfeld P, Brailovskaia J, Zhang XC, Margraf J. Universal happiness? Cross-cultural measurement invariance of scales assessing positive mental health. Psychol Assess. 2017;29: 408–421. doi: 10.1037/pas0000353 27322203

112. Van de Vijver F, Leung K. Methods and data analysis of comparative research. In: Berry JW, Poortinga YH, Pandey J, editors. Handbook of cross-cultural psychology. 2nd ed. Needham Heights, MA, US: Allyn & Bacon; 1997. pp. 257–300.

113. Tov W, Au EWM. Comparing well-being across nations: conceptual and empirical issues. In: Boniwell I, David S, Conley A, editors. Oxford handbook of happiness. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 448–464.

114. An S, Ji L-J, Marks M, Zhang Z. Two sides of emotion: exploring positivity and negativity in six basic emotions across cultures. Front Psychol. 2017;8: 610. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00610 28473791

115. Joshanloo M, Lepshokova ZKh, Panyusheva T, Natalia A, Poon W-C, Yeung VW, et al. Cross-cultural validation of fear of happiness scale across 14 national groups. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2014;45: 246–264. doi: 10.1177/0022022113505357

116. Oishi S, Graham J, Kesebir S, Galinha IC. Concepts of happiness across time and cultures. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2013;39: 559–577. doi: 10.1177/0146167213480042 23599280

117. Guerini M, Nuvolati G. Happiness, subjective and objective indicators. In: Bruni L, Porta PL, editors. Handbook of research methods and applications in happiness and quality of life. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing; 2016. pp. 400–418.


Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 10
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova

Svět praktické medicíny 1/2024 (znalostní test z časopisu)
nový kurz

Koncepce osteologické péče pro gynekology a praktické lékaře
Autoři: MUDr. František Šenk

Sekvenční léčba schizofrenie
Autoři: MUDr. Jana Hořínková

Hypertenze a hypercholesterolémie – synergický efekt léčby
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Hana Rosolová, DrSc.

Význam metforminu pro „udržitelnou“ terapii diabetu
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Milan Kvapil, CSc., MBA

Všechny kurzy
Kurzy Podcasty Doporučená témata Časopisy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#