#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Cerebellum-mediated trainability of eye and head movements for dynamic gazing


Authors: Akiyoshi Matsugi aff001;  Naoki Yoshida aff002;  Satoru Nishishita aff002;  Yohei Okada aff004;  Nobuhiko Mori aff006;  Kosuke Oku aff001;  Shinya Douchi aff008;  Koichi Hosomi aff006;  Youichi Saitoh aff006
Authors place of work: Faculty of Rehabilitation, Shijonawate Gakuen University, Hojo, Daitou City, Osaka, Japan aff001;  Department of Research, Institute of Rehabilitation Science, Tokuyukai Medical Corporation, Sakuranocho, Toyonaka City, Osaka, Japan aff002;  Department of Rehabilitation, Kansai Rehabilitation Hospital, Sakuranocho, Toyonaka City, Osaka, Japan aff003;  Faculty of Health Science, Kio University, Umami-naka, Koryo-cho, Kitakatsuragi-gun, Nara, Japan aff004;  Neurorehabilitation Research Center of Kio University, Koryo-cho, Kitakatsuragi-gun, Nara, Japan aff005;  Department of Neuromodulation and Neurosurgery, Office for University-Industry Collaboration, Osaka University, Osaka, Japan aff006;  Department of Neurosurgery, Osaka University Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka, Japan aff007;  Department of Rehabilitation, National Hospital Organization Kyoto Medical Center, Hukakusamukaihatacyo, Husimi-ku, Kyoto City, Kyoto, Japan aff008
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 14(11)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0224458

Summary

Objective

To investigate whether gaze stabilization exercises (GSEs) improve eye and head movements and whether low-frequency cerebellar repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) inhibits GSE trainability.

Methods

25 healthy adults (real rTMS, n = 12; sham rTMS, n = 13) were recruited. Real or sham rTMS was performed for 15 min (1 Hz, 900 stimulations). The center of the butterfly coil was set 1 cm below the inion in the real rTMS. Following stimulation, 10 trials of 1 min of a GSE were conducted at 1 min intervals. In the GSE, the subjects were instructed to stand upright and horizontally rotate their heads according to a beeping sound corresponding to 2 Hz and with a gaze point ahead of them. Electrooculograms were used to estimate the horizontal gaze direction of the right eye, and gyroscopic measurements were performed to estimate the horizontal head angular velocity during the GSE trials. The percentage change from the first trial of motion range of the eye and head was calculated for each measurement. The percent change of the eye/head range ratio was calculated to assess the synchronous changes of the eye and head movements as the exercise increased.

Results

Bayesian two-way analysis of variance showed that cerebellar rTMS affected the eye motion range and eye/head range ratio. A post hoc comparison (Bayesian t-test) showed evidence that the eye motion range and eye/head range ratio were reduced in the fifth, sixth, and seventh trials compared with the first trial sham stimulation condition.

Conclusions

GSEs can modulate eye movements with respect to head movements, and the cerebellum may be associated with eye–head coordination trainability for dynamic gazing during head movements.

Keywords:

Analysis of variance – Eyes – Vision – Transcranial magnetic stimulation – Eye movements – Cerebellum – Reflexes – Moths and butterflies

Introduction

Eye and head movements are necessary for accurate visual cognition in daily life [1] because the visual target image on the retina changes with head movements [2]. A low accuracy of detection of visual targets during head movements impacts daily living [3]. This accuracy, known as the dynamic gaze ability, can be improved by gaze stabilization exercises (GSEs) in healthy individuals [4]. One of the possible mechanisms for improving the dynamic gaze ability is the modulation of eye movements with respect to head movements. However, the details of this relationship are unclear.

The vestibuloocular reflex contributes to eye movements during head movements [5]. In young adults, GSEs have been shown to modulate the excitability of vestibular reflexes after only 1 min of GSE [6, 7]. Based on previous reports, it was hypothesized that eye and head movements can be changed in the second and subsequent GSE compared to the first 1 min GSE. However, it is unclear whether GSEs also improve eye and head movements during training in young adults. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate improvements in eye and head movements during GSE training.

The cerebellum is involved in the coordination of movements [8, 9], and patients with cerebellar ataxia have difficulty in performing smooth eye and head movements [10]. The cerebellum is also involved in the modulation of the vestibuloocular reflex [11, 12], and it was found that lesions in the cerebellum in monkeys also impaired the modulation of this reflex [13]. The cerebellar dorsal vermis is involved in eye and head movements [12]. Single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the inion, which can affect the cerebellum [14, 15], changes the eye–head coordination [16]. These findings indicate that the medial cerebellum, which includes the oculomotor vermis and nearby areas stimulated by TMS, may be involved in eye movements with respect to head movements. Repetitive TMS (rTMS) can modulate cortical activity beyond the stimulation period, and the possible mechanisms underlying the aftereffects of low-frequency rTMS resemble those of long-term depression [17]. rTMS over the medial cerebellum has been shown to disrupt oculomotor adaptation [18]. Based on these findings, it was hypothesized that low-frequency cerebellar rTMS disrupts the effects achieved by eye–head coordination training such as GSEs. In this study, we investigated whether eye and head movements were modulated by repetitive GSEs in the sham-rTMS condition and whether this modulation was affected by low-frequency rTMS over the medial cerebellum.

Materials and methods

Participants

25 healthy adults (mean age: 19.6 ± 0.6 years, 14 males) participated in the study. None of the participants had any history of epilepsy or other neurological diseases. The ethics committee of the Shijonawate Gakuen University approved the experimental procedures (approval code: 29–4), and the study was conducted according to the principles and guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki [19] with the understanding and written consent of each participant.

General methodology

Participants were allocated to either the sham-rTMS (n = 13) or real-rTMS (n = 12) groups in a block random method order. Sham- or real-rTMS procedures were conducted before GSE training. The range of motion of the eye and the head in the horizontal plane was measured during the GSE.

Measurement of the range of motion of the eye and head

In order to estimate the direction of gaze of the right eye in the horizontal plane, electrooculography (EOG) was carried out using JINS MEME EOG glasses (JINS Inc., Tokyo, Japan) [20]. Three dry electrodes were attached to the device and mounted on the nose bridge and nose pads as previously reported [20] (Fig 1A). A high accuracy can be achieved for the EOG data using this dry electrode method and the conventional method of attaching the wet electrode to the outside of the eyes. The former approach was shown to obtain a 6.18% higher accuracy on average compared to the conventional method [20]. In order to record the angle of the head in the horizontal plane, a gyroscope system with wireless JINS MEME glasses [20] was used (Fig 1A). The EOG and gyroscope data were synchronized using the JINS MEME system. The sampling frequency was set at 100 Hz for the EOG and gyroscope sensors. The EOG and gyroscope data were simultaneously transferred from the glasses to a smartphone device using Bluetooth during movement. The data were also transferred to a computer via the ES_R Development Kit application (JINS Inc.). The angle of gaze direction in the horizontal plane was calculated, and 0° was defined as the gaze target in front of the subject when facing the front. A positive degree angle was defined as a deviation to the right side, and a negative degree angle was defined as a deviation to the left side. The angle of the head in the horizontal plane was also calculated, and 0° was defined as the participant facing the target. A positive degree angle was defined as a deviation/rotation of the head to the right. Initially, the data were obtained using a head rotation device (see Supporting Information S1 File) and a plate indicating the angle of 22.5° to the right and left sides (see S2 Fig). The data obtained during the GSE were converted from voltage to angle. In order to estimate the range of motion of the eye and head movements between beeping sounds (BSs), the first range of motion was defined as the range between the first and second peaks, and the second range of motion was defined as the range between the second and third peaks. A single GSE trial involved 120 BSs, and thus a total of 119 points were obtained for the range of motion of the eye and head movements in each GSE trial.

Fig. 1.
Experimental setup (A) and data analysis (B). (A) The black glasses shown are the JINS MEME EOG glasses. The red ovals indicate the EOG electrodes, and the blue ovals indicate the gyroscope sensor attached within the glasses. (B) The blue line indicates the range of the head, and the red line indicates the range of the eye during the GSE. BSs are shown below the range line as short vertical lines. The BS interval was set at 500 ms for 60 s (total number of BSs = 120). The empty circles indicate the peak range, and the number “1” in the figure indicates the first peak of the range of motion. The number continues to the 120th peak. The lower panel in (B) is a schematic of the experimental procedure. The GSE interval was set at 1 min.

GSE

Before the GSE, the ability of the subjects to the target at the horizontal head rotation position was confirmed; all subjects could see the target. The subjects were then instructed to stand in an upright position and to repetitively rotate their heads to the right and left in accordance with a 2 Hz BS for 1 min while gazing at a visual target placed 1 m in front of them [4, 6, 7, 21]. The direction of the initial movement was decided by the participants. The subjects were instructed to rotate their heads with the maximum angle that can meet the gaze target [6]. 10 GSE trials were conducted at 1 min intervals (Fig 1B). EOG and gyroscope data were recorded during all tasks.

Cerebellar rTMS

The participants were asked to lie in a prone position on a bed. A magnetic stimulator (MagPro Compact; MagVenture, Farum, Denmark) was used to deliver TMS to the medial cerebellum using a butterfly coil (MC-B70; MagVenture). It has been reported in previous studies that a butterfly coil, in an eight-shaped figure, used for cerebellar stimulation, resulted in long-lasting inhibitory effects [22, 23]. It has also been shown in previous studies that positioning the center of the coil junction at 1 cm below the inion position leads to the modulation of vestibular and ocular motor functions [16, 18, 24]. The coil junction was, therefore, set at this position to stimulate the central cerebellar areas [14]. These findings indicated that rTMS with a butterfly coil can induce long-lasting inhibition of cerebellar function with respect to oculomotor adaptation. The direction of current in the coil was set downward, in order to deliver an upward current in the brain [25]. It has been shown in previous studies that this direction is effective for cerebellar stimulation [8, 2630]. The TMS intensity was set at 50% of the maximum stimulator output, similar to that in previous studies investigating cerebellar function and vestibular reflexes [18, 22, 24]. The interstimulus interval was set at 1 s, and 900 pulses were delivered [23, 31]. rTMS can lead to long-lasting aftereffects in the brain [17]. Popa et al. reported that the administration of 1 Hz rTMS (900 pulses) over the cerebellar hemisphere effect on the cerebellar output measured by a paired stimulation method (cerebellar brain inhibition) lasted for 30 min [23]. Jenkinson et al. reported that 1 Hz rTMS (120 pulses) over the inion disrupted oculomotor adaptation, and the aftereffects of rTMS lasted about 10 min [18]. Therefore, 1 min GSE was conducted 10 times immediately after the administration of the conditioning stimulation. The coil was held at a 90° angle from the scalp over the inion while delivering sham TMS [8, 32], which involves the application of auditory stimulation associated with TMS without actual brain stimulation caused by changing magnetic fields.

Electric field stimulation of the neuronal structures was performed using SimNIBS software (version 2.1.1) [33] with default head models (Fig 2). Biological tissue conductivity values were included in the software version and were set as 0.465 S/m (scalp), 0.01 S/m (bone), 0.5 S/m (eyeballs), 1.654 S/m (cerebrospinal fluid), 0.275 S/m (gray matter), and 0.126 S/m (white matter) [34]. The aforementioned parameters were set for TMS using the butterfly coil.

Simulation of the electric field induced by TMS.
Fig. 2. Simulation of the electric field induced by TMS.
Electric field induced by TMS using butterfly coils in the coronal, sagittal, and horizontal views. The scale represents normE which is the magnitude of the electric field (V/m) induced by the TMS over the site at 1 cm below the inion. The affected sites are cerebellar structures. TMS: transcranial magnetic stimulation.

Analysis

In order to estimate the degree of eye–head coordination, the eye/head ratio was calculated as the motion range of the eye divided by the motion range of the head for each motion, and the average value obtained in one GSE trial was used as the representative value for an individual. In order to estimate the change of the eye and head motion range and the eye/head ratio after 10 trials were completed, the percentage change from the first trial was calculated. For example, the percentage change of the eye motion range in the third trial was calculated as follows: (eye motion range in the first trial − eye motion range in the third trial)/(eye motion range in the first trial). Calculations were performed using Microsoft Excel for Mac (version 16.16.10; Microsoft Corp., Redmond, WA, USA) and MATLAB software (version R2014b 8.4.0; MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) in the offline mode.

Levene’s test was conducted as an assumption check to test for the equality of variances of the effect of stimulation and repetition of trials on the percent change from the first trial in the eye and head motion range and eye/head ratio, before two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted. If the variances were not equal, a nonparametric analysis (Kruskal–Wallis test) was used, and results with a p-value of <0.05 were considered to be statistically significant. Bayesian two-way ANOVA was conducted because a Bayesian hypothesis test can provide additional information to assist in the interpretation of null results, and this method is used in standalone analyses [3537]. If there was strong evidence of an alternative hypothesis, a post hoc comparison (Bayesian t-test) [38, 39] was conducted. Posterior odds were corrected for multiple testing by fixing the prior probability that the null hypothesis holds across all comparisons at 0.5 [38]. Statistical analyses were carried out using the JASP software (version 0.9.2; University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands) [39]. As in a previous study [35], we used the most common prior model as the default in this software.

We computed the predictive performance of two competing hypotheses: the null hypothesis and the alternative hypothesis, that there is an effect [35]. The Bayes factor (BF) [40] allows researchers to quantify evidence in favor of the null hypothesis [35, 41]. If BF10 > 10, we believe that there is strong evidence for accepting the alternative hypothesis [35].

Results

All participants completed all tasks. None of the participants showed any side effects in any of the examinations. Fig 3 shows a typical waveform of the gyroscope and EOG from raw data from the JINS MEME system. All of the raw data in the experiment are attached as Supporting Data. “S” indicates the sham-rTMS group, “R” indicates the real-rTMS group, and the serial number indicates the trial number of the GSE. A summary of the percent changes from the first GSE trial in the range of motion of the eye and head and the eye/head ratio is attached as Supporting Information files. Fig 4 shows the percent change in the range of motion of the eye and head and the eye/head ratio in the sham- and real-rTMS conditions as the mean and standard error. Table 1 shows the results of the tests for equality of variances (Levene’s test). The results indicate that there was no equality of variance between groups for the parametric two-way ANOVA. Therefore, we could not apply parametric two-way ANOVA, and instead nonparametric one-way ANOVA (Kruskal–Wallis test) and Bayesian two-way ANOVA were conducted. Table 2 shows the results of the Kruskal–Wallis test. There was a significant effect of conditioning stimulation observed in the eye motion range and eye/head ratio (p < 0.001), but there was no significant effect on the head motion range (p > 0.05). Table 3 shows the results of the Bayesian two-way ANOVA. The value of BF10 was >10 in both the eye motion range and eye/head ratio. Table 4 shows the results of the post hoc comparisons of the percent change in the eye and head movement and eye/head ratio between the sham-rTMS and real-rTMS groups, and BF10 > 10 in the eye motion range and eye/head ratio. Table 5 shows the results of the post hoc comparisons of the eye and head motion range and eye/head ratio between trials for each stimulation condition, and BF10 > 10 in the comparisons between the first trial and the fifth, sixth, and seventh trials in the sham-rTMS condition for the eye motion range and eye/head ratio.

Specimen waveform of the gyroscope and EOG from raw data.
Fig. 3. Specimen waveform of the gyroscope and EOG from raw data.
The blue and red lines indicate the specimen waveform of the gyroscope (Gyro_Z) and EOG (EOG_H) voltage in the horizontal plane created from raw data extracted from the JINS MEME system during the GSE.
Fig. 4.
Motion range of the head (A) and eye (B) and the eye/head ratio (C). The blank circles (real rTMS) and solid circles (sham rTMS) indicate mean values. The error bars indicate the standard error.
Tab. 1. Test for equality of variances (Levene’s test) in % change.
Test for equality of variances (Levene’s test) in % change.
Tab. 2. Kruskal–Wallis test in % change.
Kruskal–Wallis test in % change.
Tab. 3. Bayesian two-way ANOVA (model comparison) in % change.
Bayesian two-way ANOVA (model comparison) in % change.
Tab. 4. Post hoc comparison between rTMS conditions in % change.
Post hoc comparison between rTMS conditions in % change.
Tab. 5. Post hoc comparison between trials in % change.
Post hoc comparison between trials in % change.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate whether the GSE improves eye and head movements and whether low-frequency cerebellar rTMS inhibits this GSE trainability. Our results indicate that there was strong evidence that the percent change from the first trial in the range of motion of the eye and the eye/head ratio was reduced by the GSE in the sham-rTMS conditions, but this was not the case for the range of motion of the head. There was no evidence for an effect of GSEs on the head or eye motion range or eye/head ratio in the real-rTMS conditions. There was strong evidence for a reduction of the eye motion range and eye/head ratio in the fifth, sixth, and seventh trials compared with the first trial only in the sham-rTMS condition. These findings indicate that the GSE reduced the range of eye motion with respect to head movements and low-frequency rTMS over the medial cerebellum disrupted the modulation caused by training.

The visual target image on the retina deviates with head movements; therefore, anticipatory and reflexive eye movements are necessary for accurate visual cognition [1]. Accuracy in the dynamic gaze during head movements is improved by GSEs in healthy subjects [4], an observation that suggests that the dynamic gaze can be trained to detect visual targets during movements more accurately. In the present study, we asked our subjects to conduct maximal rotation of their heads in order to see the target and found that the range of eye motion during head movements was reduced by repetitive GSEs under the sham-rTMS conditions. Therefore, we speculate that a reduction in the range of eye motion may increase the accuracy of the dynamic gaze; the eye motion in the first trial might have frequently overshot the target.

The range of eye motion was changed without changing the range of head motion. Therefore, the reduction of the eye/head ratio depends upon the reduction of the eye motion range, because the head motion range does not change. One reason for this may be that the participants were trying to follow the instructions regarding head motion range.

Modulation of the eye motion range during head movements was not observed after low-frequency cerebellar rTMS. The cerebellum contributes to adaptive changes in the vestibuloocular reflex, as shown by the observation that cerebellar lesions disturb long-term adaptive changes in the vestibular reflex [11, 13]. Further, the vermal cerebellum contributes to saccadic adaptation [42]. Low-frequency cerebellar rTMS reduces cerebellar brain inhibition immediately after stimulation [23], indicating that it inhibits the excitability of the cerebellar cortex or deep nuclei. Low-frequency rTMS has been shown to disturb eye movements [18] and motor adaptation [25]. However, our result of reduced eye motion range in the sham rTMS condition indicates the overshot target, suggesting that cerebellar rTMS does not disturb eye movement immediately. Therefore, the modulation of eye movements with respect to head movements by GSEs for dynamic gaze may be associated with cerebellar function.

This study has some limitations. The sample size was small, which may account for the inequality of variances. We conducted a Bayesian analysis [35] as a complementary analysis technique. However, in order to confirm the reproducibility of the results, it will be necessary to perform additional experiments in the future, with a larger sample size. There are also some methodological considerations. In order to reduce the effect of bias in the individual abilities of eye and head movements, the participants were randomly allocated to stimulation groups. However, there may have been some bias due to the differences in the ability of the eye and head movements between individuals. We did not measure the performance of the eye and head movements during GSEs before the conditioning stimulation, because we considered that the aftereffects of GSEs before stimulation might have remained and affected the trainability of the eye and head movements. We did not measure the accuracy of detection of visual targets during rapid head rotation tasks, as was done in previous studies [4], and therefore could not estimate the change of the ability of dynamic gaze itself. We can only speculate that the dynamic gaze ability is increased as seen in the previous study [4], based on the result of the change of eye movements.

Our findings (that the range of motion of the eye is modulated with respect to head movements for dynamic gaze) may contribute to the development of a standardized methodology for vestibular rehabilitation. Patients with reduced cerebellar function may show poor response to GSEs during dynamic gazing. Further studies are needed to investigate the effects of GSEs on eye and head movements in patients with cerebellar dysfunction in clinical practice.

In conclusion, in this study, we found that training to increase the accuracy of dynamic gaze modulates eye movements with respect to head movements. However, these effects were not present after low-frequency cerebellar rTMS. This finding indicates that the cerebellum contributes to the trainability of eye movements for dynamic gaze.

Supporting information

S1 Fig [tif]
Head rotation device.

S2 Fig [tif]
Plate for gaze and head angle.

S1 File [jasp]
Statistical analysis: Test for the equality of variances (Levene’s test), Kruskal–Wallis test, Bayesian two-way ANOVA (model comparison), and post hoc comparison (between rTMS conditions) in head motion range.

S2 File [jasp]
Statistical analysis: Test for the equality of variances (Levene’s test), Kruskal–Wallis test, Bayesian two-way ANOVA (model comparison), and post hoc comparison (between rTMS conditions) in eye motion range.

S3 File [jasp]
Statistical analysis: Test for the equality of variances (Levene’s test), Kruskal–Wallis test, Bayesian two-way ANOVA (model comparison), and post hoc comparisons between rTMS conditions in eye/head ratio.

S4 File [jasp]
Statistical analysis: Post hoc comparisons between trials in head motion range of sham rTMS.

S5 File [jasp]
Statistical analysis: Post hoc comparisons between trials in head motion range of real rTMS.

S6 File [jasp]
Statistical analysis: Post hoc comparisons between trials in eye motion range of sham rTMS.

S7 File [jasp]
Statistical analysis: Post hoc comparisons between trials in eye motion range of real rTMS.

S8 File [jasp]
Statistical analysis: Post hoc comparisons between trials in eye/head ratio of sham rTMS.

S9 File [jasp]
Statistical analysis: Post hoc comparisons between trials in eye/head ratio of real rTMS.


Zdroje

1. Fang Y, Nakashima R, Matsumiya K, Kuriki I, Shioiri S. Eye-head coordination for visual cognitive processing. PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0121035. Epub 2015/03/24. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121035 25799510; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4370616.

2. Noda H. Mossy fibres sending retinal-slip, eye, and head velocity signals to the flocculus of the monkey. J Physiol. 1986;379:39–60. Epub 1986/10/01. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016240 3559999; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1182884.

3. Mitsutake T, Sakamoto M, Ueta K, Oka S, Horikawa E. Poor gait performance is influenced with decreased vestibulo-ocular reflex in poststroke patients. Neuroreport. 2017;28(12):745–8. Epub 2017/06/24. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000841 28640006.

4. Morimoto H, Asai Y, Johnson EG, Lohman EB, Khoo K, Mizutani Y, et al. Effect of oculo-motor and gaze stability exercises on postural stability and dynamic visual acuity in healthy young adults. Gait Posture. 2011;33(4):600–3. Epub 2011/02/19. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.01.016 21334899.

5. Schmid R, Zambarbieri D. The role of the vestibular system in eye-head coordination and the generation of vestibular nystagmus. Adv Otorhinolaryngol. 1988;41:89–94. Epub 1988/01/01. doi: 10.1159/000416037 3265010.

6. Matsugi A, Ueta Y, Oku K, Okuno K, Tamaru Y, Nomura S, et al. Effect of gaze-stabilization exercises on vestibular function during postural control. Neuroreport. 2017;28(8):439–43. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000776 28368883.

7. Ueta Y, Matsugi A, Oku K, Okuno K, Tamaru Y, Nomura S, et al. Gaze stabilization exercises derive sensory reweighting of vestibular for postural control. J Phys Ther Sci. 2017;29(9):1494–6. Epub 2017/09/15. doi: 10.1589/jpts.29.1494 28931974; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5599807.

8. Matsugi A, Iwata Y, Mori N, Horino H, Hiraoka K. Long latency electromyographic response induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation over the cerebellum preferentially appears during continuous visually guided manual tracking task. Cerebellum. 2013;12(2):147–54. doi: 10.1007/s12311-012-0402-6 22806979.

9. Thach WT, Goodkin HP, Keating JG. The cerebellum and the adaptive coordination of movement. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1992;15:403–42. Epub 1992/01/01. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002155 1575449.

10. Panouilleres M, Frismand S, Sillan O, Urquizar C, Vighetto A, Pelisson D, et al. Saccades and eye-head coordination in ataxia with oculomotor apraxia type 2. Cerebellum. 2013;12(4):557–67. Epub 2013/03/12. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0463-1 23475383.

11. Ito M. Cerebellar learning in the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Trends Cogn Sci. 1998;2(9):313–21. 21227227.

12. Shinmei Y, Yamanobe T, Fukushima J, Fukushima K. Purkinje cells of the cerebellar dorsal vermis: simple-spike activity during pursuit and passive whole-body rotation. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87(4):1836–49. Epub 2002/04/04. doi: 10.1152/jn.00150.2001 11929905.

13. Miles FA, Eighmy BB. Long-term adaptive changes in primate vestibuloocular reflex. I. Behavioral observations. J Neurophysiol. 1980;43(5):1406–25. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.43.5.1406 6768851.

14. Hardwick RM, Lesage E, Miall RC. Cerebellar Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation: The Role of Coil Geometry and Tissue Depth. Brain Stimul. 2014. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.04.009 24924734.

15. Matsugi A. Do changes in spinal reflex excitability elicited by transcranial magnetic stimulation differ based on the site of cerebellar stimulation? Somatosens Mot Res. 2018;35(2):80–5. Epub 2018/05/08. doi: 10.1080/08990220.2018.1465403 29732943.

16. Nagel M, Zangemeister WH. The effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation over the cerebellum on the synkinesis of coordinated eye and head movements. J Neurol Sci. 2003;213(1–2):35–45. Epub 2003/07/23. doi: 10.1016/s0022-510x(03)00145-x 12873753.

17. Klomjai W, Katz R, Lackmy-Vallee A. Basic principles of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and repetitive TMS (rTMS). Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2015;58(4):208–13. Epub 2015/09/01. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2015.05.005 26319963.

18. Jenkinson N, Miall RC. Disruption of saccadic adaptation with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the posterior cerebellum in humans. Cerebellum. 2010;9(4):548–55. Epub 2010/07/29. doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0193-6 20665254; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2996540.

19. World Medical A. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA. 2013;310(20):2191–4. Epub 2013/10/22. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053 24141714.

20. Nathaniel Barbara TAC. Interfacing with a speller using EOG glasses. 016 IEEE International Conference on Systems Man and Cybernetics (SMC). 2016:1069–74. doi: 10.1109/SMC.2016.7844384

21. Bhardwaj V VM. Effectiveness of gaze stability exercise on balance in healthy elderly population. Int J Physiother Res. 2014;2(4):642–7.

22. Haarmeier T, Kammer T. Effect of TMS on oculomotor behavior but not perceptual stability during smooth pursuit eye movements. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20(9):2234–43. Epub 2010/01/13. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp285 20064941.

23. Popa T, Russo M, Meunier S. Long-lasting inhibition of cerebellar output. Brain Stimul. 2010;3(3):161–9. Epub 2010/07/17. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2009.10.001 20633445.

24. Zangemeister WH, Nagel M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation over the cerebellum delays predictive head movements in the coordination of gaze. Acta oto-laryngologica Supplementum. 2001;545:140–4. Epub 2001/10/27. doi: 10.1080/000164801750388324 11677729.

25. van Dun K, Bodranghien F, Manto M, Marien P. Targeting the Cerebellum by Noninvasive Neurostimulation: a Review. Cerebellum. 2017;16(3):695–741. Epub 2016/12/30. doi: 10.1007/s12311-016-0840-7 28032321.

26. Matsugi A, Okada Y. Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation modulates the effect of cerebellar transcranial magnetic stimulation on the excitability of spinal reflex. Neurosci Res. 2019. Epub 2019/02/23. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2019.01.012 30794822.

27. Matsugi A, Mori N, Uehara S, Kamata N, Oku K, Mukai K, et al. Task dependency of the long-latency facilitatory effect on the soleus H-reflex by cerebellar transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroreport. 2014;25(17):1375–80. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000275 25325350.

28. Matsugi A, Kikuchi Y, Kaneko K, Seko Y, Odagaki M. Cerebellar transcranial magnetic stimulation facilitates excitability of spinal reflex, but does not affect cerebellar inhibition and facilitation in spinocerebellar ataxia. Neuroreport. 2018;29(10):808–13. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000001036 29659444; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5999368.

29. Matsugi A, Kamata N, Tanaka T, Hiraoka K. Long latency fluctuation of the finger movement evoked by cerebellar TMS during visually guided manual tracking task. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2012;56(3):193–200. 23734432.

30. Tanaka H, Matsugi A, Okada Y. The effects of imaginary voluntary muscle contraction and relaxation on cerebellar brain inhibition. Neurosci Res. 2017. Epub 2017/11/14. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2017.11.004 29154805.

31. Fierro B, Giglia G, Palermo A, Pecoraro C, Scalia S, Brighina F. Modulatory effects of 1 Hz rTMS over the cerebellum on motor cortex excitability. Exp Brain Res. 2007;176(3):440–7. Epub 2006/08/19. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0628-y 16917771.

32. Hiraoka K, Horino K, Yagura A, Matsugi A. Cerebellar TMS evokes a long latency motor response in the hand during a visually guided manual tracking task. Cerebellum. 2010;9(3):454–60. doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0187-4 20549404.

33. Thielscher A, Antunes A, Saturnino GB. Field modeling for transcranial magnetic stimulation: A useful tool to understand the physiological effects of TMS? Conference proceedings: Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society Annual Conference. 2015;2015:222–5. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2015.7318340 26736240.

34. Boayue NM, Csifcsak G, Puonti O, Thielscher A, Mittner M. Head models of healthy and depressed adults for simulating the electric fields of non-invasive electric brain stimulation. F1000Res. 2018;7:704. Epub 2018/12/12. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.15125.2 30505431; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6241565.2.

35. Hoekstra R, Monden R, van Ravenzwaaij D, Wagenmakers EJ. Bayesian reanalysis of null results reported in medicine: Strong yet variable evidence for the absence of treatment effects. PLoS One. 2018;13(4):e0195474. Epub 2018/04/26. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0195474 29694370; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5919013.

36. van Ravenzwaaij D, Monden R, Tendeiro JN, Ioannidis JPA. Bayes factors for superiority, non-inferiority, and equivalence designs. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2019;19(1):71. Epub 2019/03/31. doi: 10.1186/s12874-019-0699-7 30925900; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6441196.

37. Dienes Z, Coulton S, Heather N. Using Bayes factors to evaluate evidence for no effect: examples from the SIPS project. Addiction. 2018;113(2):240–6. Epub 2017/08/15. doi: 10.1111/add.14002 28804980.

38. Peter H. Westfall WOJaJMU. A Bayesian Perspective on the Bonferroni Adjustment. Biometrika. 1997;84(2):9.

39. Team J. JASP (Version 0.10.1)[Computer software]. 2019.

40. Hobbs BP, Carlin BP. Practical Bayesian design and analysis for drug and device clinical trials. J Biopharm Stat. 2008;18(1):54–80. Epub 2007/12/29. doi: 10.1080/10543400701668266 18161542.

41. Zaslavsky BG. Bayesian hypothesis testing in two-arm trials with dichotomous outcomes. Biometrics. 2013;69(1):157–63. Epub 2012/09/26. doi: 10.1111/j.1541-0420.2012.01806.x 23002906.

42. Soetedjo R, Kojima Y, Fuchs AF. How cerebellar motor learning keeps saccades accurate. J Neurophysiol. 2019;121(6):2153–62. Epub 2019/04/18. doi: 10.1152/jn.00781.2018 30995136; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6620692.


Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 11
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova

Svět praktické medicíny 1/2024 (znalostní test z časopisu)
nový kurz

Koncepce osteologické péče pro gynekology a praktické lékaře
Autoři: MUDr. František Šenk

Sekvenční léčba schizofrenie
Autoři: MUDr. Jana Hořínková

Hypertenze a hypercholesterolémie – synergický efekt léčby
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Hana Rosolová, DrSc.

Význam metforminu pro „udržitelnou“ terapii diabetu
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Milan Kvapil, CSc., MBA

Všechny kurzy
Kurzy Podcasty Doporučená témata Časopisy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#