#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Is it really always only the others who are to blame? GP’s view on medical overuse. A questionnaire study


Authors: Maximilian Pausch aff001;  Angela Schedlbauer aff002;  Maren Weiss aff003;  Thomas Kuehlein aff002;  Susann Hueber aff002
Authors place of work: Faculty of Medicine, Friedrich-Alexander-University Erlangen-Nürnberg (FAU), Erlangen, Germany aff001;  Institute of General Practice, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany aff002;  Institute of Psychology, Friedrich-Alexander-University Erlangen-Nürnberg (FAU), Erlangen, Germany aff003
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 15(1)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227457

Summary

Background

Medical overuse is a common problem in health care. Preventing unnecessary medicine is one of the main tasks of General Practice, so called quaternary prevention. We aimed to capture the current opinion of German General Practitioners (GPs) to medical overuse.

Methods

A quantitative online study was conducted. The questionnaire was developed based on a qualitative study and literature search. GPs were asked to estimate prevalence of medical overuse as well as to evaluate drivers and solutions of medical overuse. GPs in Bavaria were recruited via email (750 addresses). A descriptive data analysis was performed. Additionally the association between doctors’ attitudes and (1) demographic variables and (2) interest in campaigns against medical overuse was assessed.

Results

Response rate was 18%. The mean age was 54 years, 79% were male and 68% have worked as GP longer than 15 years. Around 38% of medical services were considered as medical overuse and nearly half of the GPs (47%) judged medical overuse to be the more important problem than medical underuse. Main drivers were seen in “patients´ expectations” (76%), “lack of a primary care system” (61%) and “defensive medicine” (53%), whereas “disregard of evidence/guidelines” (15%) and “economic pressure on the side of the doctor” (13%) were not weighted as important causes. Demographic variables did not have an important impact on GPs´ response pattern. GPs interested in campaigns like “Choosing Wisely” showed a higher awareness for medical overuse, although these campaigns were only known by 50% of the respondents.

Discussion

Medical overuse is an important issue for GPs. Main drivers were searched and found outside their own sphere of responsibility. Campaigns as “Choosing Wisely” seem to have a positive effect on GPs attitude, but knowledge is still limited.

Keywords:

Medical doctors – Physicians – Health economics – Preventive medicine – Qualitative studies – Questionnaires – Decision making – Primary care

Introduction

Medical overuse is a common problem in health care [1, 2]. Despite frequent discussions in public, politics and the medical community, there is still no consistent concept for defining and measuring medical overuse [3, 4]. Medical overuse is often described as “a health care service [that] is provided under circumstances in which its potential for harm exceeds the possible benefit” [5]. This rather simplified definition only corresponds to obviously ineffective services. However, many services are in a “nebulous grey zone, where evidence is lacking or weak.” [2]. In this grey zone, patients’ and physicians’ attitudes and beliefs might play the ultimate role in determining and tackling with low value care [6].

Much of the current literature pays particular attention to drivers and solutions of medical overuse [see for example 7, 8, 9]. Factors promoting medical overuse in general practice were attributed to internal factors such as physicians’ need for reassurance and the belief that action is better than inaction [10]. Also, cognitive biases in medical decision making and an insufficient ability of dealing with uncertainty seem to play an important role [11, 12]. New medical technology and its general availability are frequently misleading physicians to use it whether appropriate or not [13]. Causes of medical overuse were also attributed to external factors such as patient expectations [14] and fear of litigation resulting in defensive medicine [15].

Estimations of the extent of medical overuse range from 10% to 30% of the total expenditure in the US healthcare system depending on the respective definition and research method [16, 17]. In 2011, between $158 and $226 billion were spent for overtreatment [1719]. Beside the economic burden, there is an important medical impact. Unnecessary investigation and treatment will frequently result in psychological and physical harm [20].

In light of lowering risk-factor thresholds and disease mongering the prevention of medical overuse–also named quaternary prevention—is getting more and more important [21]. Supporting physicians in this task is the important purpose of campaigns like “Choosing Wisely”[22]. Previous studies have shown that “Choosing Wisely” can have a positive impact on physicians’ attitudes, but are still not known enough by the vast majority of practicing doctors [23, 24].

The GP’s role as a gate-keeper is weakened considerably in the German health care system, where patients have direct access to specialists without the need to be referred. Nevertheless, GPs might be in a central position for preventing medical overuse, considering that it is easier not to initiate a cascade of diagnostic tests and their therapeutic consequences, rather than trying to stop it when it is in the full run [25, 26].

In a previous study, we qualitatively explored perceptions and opinions of medical overuse amongst German GPs [27]. They perceived medical overuse as a common problem in the German health care system, mainly caused by drivers like defensive medicine, lack of a primary care system and patients’ expectations. Solutions proposed were reducing defensive medicine, focusing on shared-decision-making and conducting stepwise diagnostic investigation. In this current study, we wanted to quantify these data. The primary aim was to capture the current opinion of GPs regarding medical overuse. The secondary aim was to assess the association between doctors’ attitudes and (1) demographic variables, and (2) campaigns´ awareness and interest.

Materials and methods

Study design and sample

We conducted an online questionnaire study. The study is reported following the STROBE and CHERRIES statement [28, 29]. The questionnaire was developed following the generalization model described by Mayring [30]. In order to gather general conclusions about a specific topic, the results of a qualitative study can be used to develop a quantitative study.

Referring to the results of the qualitative study by Alber et al [31], we decided to consider the following domains of medical overuse as to be important: perceived relevance, drivers of medical overuse and solutions. In the development of the items, both findings of a literature search [4, 16, 3238] and experts’ opinion were included. The software SurveyMonkey was used for programming the questionnaire [39].

A pre-test of the questionnaire evaluating its design and content was performed with nine physicians and two medical students. Pre-tests were carried out using the “thinking aloud” method [40]. For the recruiting of participants, the following inclusion criteria were applied: Qualified GPs and GP trainees, specialist doctors in internal medicine or physicians without specialist training working as primary care physicians.

The link to the questionnaire was sent via email together with an invitation to participate. Email addresses were selected from the registry of GPs in the Bavarian Association of Statutory Health Insurance Physicians, short Bavarian ASIP. (The Bavarian ASIP is one of the healthcare system’s self-administration bodies, responsible for ensuring that outpatient medical treatment is provided and for the distribution of payments to physicians in ambulatory care). In the case of missing email addresses in the ASIP registry, addresses were searched via Internet (Google search) resulting in a total of 750 addresses.

An email containing a link to the survey, a description of study’s objective, information on data handling (anonymity), the informed consent statement and the invitation to participate was delivered to GPs in North and East Bavaria on 20 June 2017. A reminder was sent one week later. Data collection was closed 18 days after the first mail.

Ethical approval was granted by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nürnberg (91_17 B, 07.04.2017).

Measures

The questionnaire consisted of two parts. Firstly, we collected demographic variables such as gender, age, professional title and experience, area of work and practice volume. Secondly, we asked 36 questions on the following topics in the main section of our questionnaire:

A. General assessment of medical overuse

  • 1

    Estimate of the prevalence of medical overuse in health services: “How high would you estimate the percentage of medical overuse in Germany at the moment?” on a scale between 0 and 100%.

  • 2

    Perceived need for action: “Where do you currently see more need for action regarding the quality of treatment for our patients?” Decision has to be made on a visual analogue scale between medical underuse and medical overuse.

  • 3

    Perceived causes for medical overuse: Out of a list of eight suspected causes such as patient expectation, marketing of the pharmaceutical industry, disease mongering, the three most important sources of medical overuse should be selected. There was no ranking option, just a selection mode.

  • 4

    Campaigns’ familiarity and use: selection of “Choosing wisely” [41], “Smarter medicine” [42], “Less is more” [43], “Klug entscheiden” [44], “Quaternary prevention” [25] and “None of the named campaigns”.

B. Personal attitude towards medical overuse

Participants were asked to make a decision on a six-point Likert scale. Endpoints ranged from “I completely disagree” to “I totally agree” (topic five and eight) or from”not correct at all” to “fully correct”(topic six and seven).

  • 5

    Tendency to justify medical overuse: 13 items on medical decision making in everyday practice, e.g., “Patients with acute low back pain seem to be dissatisfied when symptoms are not clarified by imaging techniques.”

  • 6

    Perceived relevance of medical overuse: Six items, e.g., “I know patients being harmed by medical overuse.”

  • 7

    Evaluation of approaches to prevent medical overuse: Six items, e.g., “As a doctor, one should talk to one’s patients about the costs of tests and medication”.

  • 8

    Evaluation of “Choosing Wisely” advices for family physicians: Six items, e.g., “Don’t perform imaging for low back pain within the first six weeks unless red flags are present.”

Reasons for the questionnaire design chosen: GPs were asked to estimate the prevalence of medical overuse at the beginning of the survey (rather than at the end) in order to avoid bias caused by confronting them with the subject of medical overuse throughout answering the questionnaire. A Likert scale with an even number of categories was chosen so that our respondents had no neutral category and were forced to pick an option for or against an item [45]. Items were randomly assigned for each participant in the topics to prevent hidden biases due to the same item sequence presentation (order effect bias) [46]. In question three, participants had to select three of the most substantial causes suspected for medical overuse–so called forced choice question [45]. It was not possible to flip backwards in the questionnaire and modify items retrospectively in order to prevent social desirability bias [47]. GPs should not be able to modify answers based on an upcoming feeling that the authors have a preference which they want to hear. The full questionnaire is available as supplementary file.

Data analysis

Only completely answered questionnaires were included in the analysis. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS, version 24 (IBM Statistics). The processing of the data was carried out in two steps: A descriptive analysis followed by an evaluation of group differences. For all items on the six-point Likert scale mean and standard deviation were computed. A box plot was chosen for graphical presentation.

Participants were grouped according to their demographic variables including gender (male vs. female), age (≤ 50 years vs. > 50 years) and practice volume (patients treated per physician per quarter (≤ 1000 vs. > 1000). In addition, respondents were grouped according to being interested or not being interested in the mentioned campaigns resulting in two groups: “Campaign interested GPs”, who had already heard at least of one campaign and “non-interested GPs”, who had heard of none. A similar approach was chosen by Kost et al. [24].

Differences between groups were assessed in regard to the main topics of the questionnaire: “General assessment of medical overuse” (section A) and “Personal attitudes towards medical overuse” (section B). For section B, a sum score for each of the four subtopics was built. A sum score is defined as the sum of all numerical item values per participant summed up in one topic of the questionnaire [42]. For a reasonable and homogeneous interpretation of the sum score, all items must have the same item polarization [43]. Therefore, we exchanged the scale endpoints of the items 5.1, 5.12, 6.1, 6.3 and 6.4. Cronbachs Alpha was used to test whether a sum score for each subtopic was reasonable (see below). Cronbachs Alpha is low for two subtopics, which in part might be due to the low number of items per subtopic. A higher sum score indicated:

  • higher tendency to justify medical overuse (topic 5): Cronbachs α = 0.64, sum score range (R) = 13–78,

  • higher rating of relevance and frequency (topic 6): Cronbachs α = 0.55, sum score range (R) = 6–36

  • higher acceptance of proposed solutions for preventing medical overuse (topic 7): Cronbachs α = 0.39, sum score range (R) = 6–36

  • higher acceptance of Choosing wisely advices (topic 8): Cronbachs α = 0.71, sum score range (R) = 6–36

Groups were compared using Students t-Tests.

Results

Descriptive analysis

Response rate: We invited 750 practices via email to participate and received 155 (21%) questionnaires of which 135 (87%) were fully completed, resulting in a response rate of 18% that could be used for data analysis.

The mean age of participants was 54 years (SD = 8), 79% were male and 92% were trained GPs. The majority (68%) had professional experience as a GP for over 15 years. Approximately half of the participants regarded their doctor’s office to be located in an urban area (51%) and almost two thirds (65%) had a practice volume of more than 1.000 patients per quarter. Demographic data can also be seen in Table 1.

Tab. 1. Demographic data of respondents.
Demographic data of respondents.

Information on the demographics about the entire population of GPs in Northern Bavaria are provided by the Bavarian Association of Statutory Health Insurance Physicians (Bavarian ASIP, described above). In the entire population, mean age is 55.3 years and 64% are male. That is, our sample was only slightly younger and had higher percentage of men.

A. General assessment of medical overuse

Frequency estimation of medical overuse: Around 38% of medical services were considered as overuse.

Perceived need for action: Medical underuse or overuse: Nearly half (47%) of the respondents judged medical overuse to be the more important problem. For 28% it was more important to tackle medical underuse.

Presumed causes for medical overuse: Factors contemplated most frequently were “patients´ expectations” (76%), “lack of a primary care system” (61%), and “defensive medicine” (53%), whereas “disregard of evidence/guidelines” (15%) and “economic pressure on the side of the doctor” (13%) appeared least frequently. Results can also be seen in Table 2.

Tab. 2. Frequency of presumed causes for medical overuse ().
Frequency of presumed causes for medical overuse ().

Campaign awareness and interest: Half of the participants (50%) had never heard of the campaigns. “Choosing wisely” was known to 32%, of whom 88% stated that they had also used it. The German offshoot “Klug entscheiden” was known to 30%, of which 80% had already taken a closer look at the respective items. Results can be seen in Table 3.

Tab. 3. Campaign awareness and interest.
Campaign awareness and interest.

B. Personal attitude towards medical overuse

Selected results are shown below. All results are depicted in Figs 1 to 3.

Descriptive analysis of items on personal attitudes to medical overuse (Item 5.1 to Item 5.13).
Fig. 1. Descriptive analysis of items on personal attitudes to medical overuse (Item 5.1 to Item 5.13).
For each item, mean value and standard deviation are presented. A box plot for each item is shown in the last column. It consists of the minimum and maximum, the interquartile range and the median. The black dot represents the mean.
Descriptive analysis of items on personal attitudes to medical overuse (Item 6.1 to Item 6.6 and Item 7.1 to Item 7.6).
Fig. 2. Descriptive analysis of items on personal attitudes to medical overuse (Item 6.1 to Item 6.6 and Item 7.1 to Item 7.6).
For each item, mean value and standard deviation are presented. A box plot for each item is shown in the last column. It consists of the minimum and maximum, the interquartile range and the median. The black dot represents the mean.
Descriptive analysis of items on personal attitudes to medical overuse (Item 8.1 to Item 8.6).
Fig. 3. Descriptive analysis of items on personal attitudes to medical overuse (Item 8.1 to Item 8.6).
For each item, mean value and standard deviation are presented. A box plot for each item is shown in the last column. It consists of the minimum and maximum, the interquartile range and the median. The black dot represents the mean.

Tendency to justify medical overuse: Most of the doctors perceived that patients with unspecific back pain seemed dissatisfied if their symptoms were not being checked via diagnostic imaging (Item 5.4, M = 4.1, SD = 1.4). The majority agreed that patients associated competence with performing more diagnostic tests (Item 5.5, M = 3.8, SD = 1.3) and that they want to figure out the causes for symptoms as soon as possible (Item 5.13, M = 4.7, SD = 1.1.). A high proportion admit that defensive medicine leads to medical overuse (Item 5.8, M = 4.7, SD = 1.3). A majority of GPs agreed to the statement that they already have “decided against therapeutic measures, even though the newly implemented lower threshold would have suggested treatment of some form" (Item 5.12, M = 4.5; SD = 1.6).

Perceived relevance of medical overuse: A majority was aware of incidental findings and related problems (Item 6.5, M = 4.2; SD = 1.4). Doctors mostly agreed to the statement of knowing patients being harmed by medical overuse (Item 6.6, M = 4.3; SD = 1.7). At the same time, a high proportion acknowledged that “The topic of medical overuse is mostly overlooked in medical discussions.” (Item 6.1, M = 4.1; SD = 1.5).

Evaluation of approaches to prevent medical overuse: GPs supported further action of politics by mostly agreeing with the item “More political engagement is necessary in order to counteract medical overuse effectively.”(Item 7.2, M = 4.6; SD = 1.4). The gatekeeper role was considered important to almost all respondents (Item 7.3, M = 5.2; SD = 1.0). Finally, a high proportion emphasized the role of long professional experience in avoiding medical overuse (Item 7.4; M = 5.1; SD = 1.0).

Evaluation of “Choosing Wisely” advices for family physicians: Recommendations were well acknowledged as illustrated by high agreement rates for most of the recommendations. However, fewer doctors agreed to the recommendation “Don’t obtain blood chemistry panels or perform urine analyses for screening in asymptomatic, healthy adults” (Item 8.5; M = 3.1; SD = 1.8).

Group differences

Demographic variables: Most demographic variables did not show a significant influence on response patterns. Only a higher number of younger doctors (≤ 50 years) agreed to approaches to prevent medical overuse (t = 2.145, CI = [0.129; 3.191], p = 0.034). Results are also shown in Table 4.

Tab. 4. Results of general assessment of medical overuse and personal attitudes towards medical overuse of groups differing in gender, age and practice volume.
Results of general assessment of medical overuse and personal attitudes towards medical overuse of groups differing in gender, age and practice volume.

Campaign awareness and interest: GPs interested in campaigns rated the necessity to avoid medical overuse higher than non-interested GPs (campaign interested GPs = 54%, non-interested GPs = 40%, p = 0.023). Non-interested GPs showed a higher tendency to justify medical overuse as compared to campaign interested GPs (sum score: non-interested GPs = 50 vs. campaign interested GPs = 47; p = 0.033). Campaign interested GPs showed a higher awareness of the relevance of medical overuse (sum score: non-interested GPs = 21, campaign interested GPs = 25, p = 0.001) and approval of “Choosing Wisely” recommendations (sum score: non-interested GPs = 26 vs. campaign interested GPs = 30, p = 0.001). Both groups rated patients´ expectations, lack of a primary care system and defensive medicine as most influential causes for medical overuse. The frequency of medical procedures considered as overuse was estimated only slightly higher by campaign interested GPs (campaign interested = 39% vs. non-interested GPs = 36%, p = 0.353). Results are depicted in Table 5.

Tab. 5. Results of general assessment of medical overuse and personal attitudes towards medical overuse of campaign-interested GPs vs. non-interested GPs.
Results of general assessment of medical overuse and personal attitudes towards medical overuse of campaign-interested GPs vs. non-interested GPs.

Discussion

Medical overuse was seen as a relevant problem that needs to be tackled, even though GPs agreed that overuse is barely discussed in the medical community. Also, knowledge of campaigns addressing medical overuse was limited. Nonetheless, recommendations given by the “Choosing Wisely” campaign were well accepted. Main drivers were seen in patients’ expectations, lack of a primary care system and defensive medicine. GPs did not rate the disregard of evidence by physicians as a main driver. Instead they accused a pressure to act by patients not accepting a “wait and see” approach. Active medical performing seems also to be attributed with more competence than non-acting. GPs agreed that incidental findings and widening of disease boundaries are a serious problem. Asked about possible solutions, GPs considered it a main political task to create conditions that help to prevent medical overuse. An important answer to tackle medical overuse was considered in the implementation of a primary care system.

Similar to findings from other countries, GPs in Germany regarded medical overuse as an important issue [35, 36]. Their conviction that patients are more satisfied and that doctors are considered as more competent through actions is well known as commission bias in medical decision making [48]. A study with patients suffering from chronic pain revealed that physicians’ refusal to prescribe opioids was attributed to distrust or lack of caring by some patients [49]. It seems possible that GPs are afraid that “watchful waiting” might also be attributed in a similar way and as a consequence interferes with the patient-doctor relationship. This fear together with fulfilling perceived patients’ expectations might lead to unnecessary medical procedures.

Consistent with other studies, main drivers of overuse were seen in factors outside GPs own responsibility [35, 36, 50]. It is known that attributing causes for failures or negative events to others helps in self-serving but is likely to prevent self-responsibility and behavior change [51]. It seems that medical overuse is mainly discussed by politics, health insurances and academic institutions, rather than by practicing physicians. The results of our study point to the importance of deeply involving practicing physicians in the discussion and in the process of defining causes and solutions of medical overuse. More effort will be needed to strengthen self-responsibility and commitment of GPs in a way that they feel confident to contribute to this issue.

The limited awareness of GPs for campaigns against medical overuse is not unusual [50, 52]. In our study, campaign interested GPs represented a more critical point of view with higher awareness of medical overuse. Asked about agreement with the recommendations of the “Choosing Wisely” campaigns, GPs mostly agreed with them. In addition, GPs agreed that guidelines should explicitly point out not to perform certain therapeutic or diagnostic services. Still it is somewhat surprising that the recommendation not to screen asymptomatic healthy individuals with blood or urine analyses was rejected. We assume that health checks are not only used to screen otherwise asymptomatic adults but were also seen as a possibility to check for medically unexplained symptoms. As long as nothing is found—which is usually the case—the patient is at ease. It seems that the contents of the campaigns are widely accepted in general, but need more and effective promotion to be implemented in everyday practice. As the perception of medical overuse was not affected by demographic variables, it seems that the attitude towards it is rather a question of personality than a question of age or gender. We might have to put more effort in identifying those early adopters in order to support them.

Strengths and limitations: Our study represents a first comprehensive empirical investigation to describe GPs’ views on medical overuse in Germany. The questionnaire was developed carefully, systematically and following the results of a qualitative study [27]. Response rate of 18% was limited in comparison to other surveys [36, 52]. Reasons might be the lack of incentive and/or a short recruitment time. Demographic variables were relatively close to the real distribution in the respective regions. GPs in our sample were slightly younger and proportion of men was higher. Nonetheless, findings should be interpreted with caution as generalization might be reduced. The voluntary participation and lack of financial compensation for participation can lead to a potential bias as it can be assumed that mostly GPs who are interested in medical overuse might have been more likely to respond to our questionnaire, skewing the sample. The study design did not allow observing the real behavior of the participants, but enables us to draw conclusions from attitudes to behavior.

Conclusion

German GPs perceive that medical overuse is a problem that needs to be solved. However, causes and solutions were mainly seen outside their own responsibility and reach. Our findings lead to the conclusion that GPs’ own contributions to medical overuse are neglected by them, maybe unconsciously. More effort is needed to increase awareness for medical overuse. Increasing awareness amongst medical students during medical education might be an important step forward. Also, greater efforts are needed to enhance self-efficacy and ownership regarding medical overuse.

Supporting information

S1 File [docx]

S2 File [docx]

S1 Data [sav]


Zdroje

1. Welch HG, Schwartz L, Woloshin S. Overdiagnosed: making people sick in the pursuit of health: Beacon Press; 2011.

2. Brownlee S, Chalkidou K, Doust J, Elshaug AG, Glasziou P, Heath I, et al. Evidence for overuse of medical services around the world. The Lancet. 2017;390(10090):156–68.

3. Carter SM, Rogers W, Heath I, Degeling C, Doust J, Barratt A. The challenge of overdiagnosis begins with its definition. BMJ (Clinical research ed). 2015;350:h869. Epub 2015/03/06. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h869 25740625.

4. Hofmann B. Diagnosing overdiagnosis: conceptual challenges and suggested solutions. European journal of epidemiology. 2014;29(9):599–604. doi: 10.1007/s10654-014-9920-5 24880635

5. Chassin MR, Galvin RW. The urgent need to improve health care quality: Institute of Medicine National Roundtable on Health Care Quality. Jama. 1998;280(11):1000–5. doi: 10.1001/jama.280.11.1000 9749483

6. Kelly MP, Heath I, Howick J, Greenhalgh T. The importance of values in evidence-based medicine. BMC Medical Ethics. 2015;16(1):69. doi: 10.1186/s12910-015-0063-3 26459219

7. Morgan DJ, Brownlee S, Leppin AL, Kressin N, Dhruva SS, Levin L, et al. Setting a research agenda for medical overuse. BMJ (Clinical research ed). 2015;351:h4534. Epub 2015/08/27. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h4534 26306661.

8. Kale MS, Korenstein D. Overdiagnosis in primary care: framing the problem and finding solutions. BMJ (Clinical research ed). 2018;362:k2820. Epub 2018/08/16. doi: 10.1136/bmj.k2820 30108054.

9. Morgan DJ, Dhruva SS, Coon ER, Wright SM, Korenstein D. 2017 Update on Medical Overuse: A Systematic Review. JAMA internal medicine. 2018;178(1):110–5. Epub 2017/10/04. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2017.4361 28973402.

10. Unnecessary tests and procedures in the health care system: Choosing Wisely; [cited 2017]. http://www.choosingwisely.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/04/Final-Choosing-Wisely-Survey-Report.pdf.

11. Gigerenzer G, Gaissmaier W, Kurz-Milcke E, Schwartz LM, Woloshin S. Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2007;8(2):53–96. Epub 2007/11/01. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x 26161749.

12. Saposnik G, Redelmeier D, Ruff CC, Tobler PN. Cognitive biases associated with medical decisions: a systematic review. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2016;16(1):138. Epub 2016/11/05. doi: 10.1186/s12911-016-0377-1 27809908.

13. Hofmann BM. Too much technology. BMJ (Clinical research ed). 2015;350:h705. Epub 2015/02/18. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h705 25687230.

14. Hoffmann TC, Del Mar C. Patients’ expectations of the benefits and harms of treatments, screening, and tests: a systematic review. JAMA internal medicine. 2015;175(2):274–86. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.6016 25531451

15. Bishop TF, Federman AD, Keyhani S. Physicians’ views on defensive medicine: a national survey. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2010;170(12):1081–3. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2010.155 20585077

16. Morgan DJ, Brownlee S, Leppin AL, Kressin N, Dhruva SS, Levin L, et al. Setting a research agenda for medical overuse. BMJ: British Medical Journal. 2015;351.

17. Berwick DM, Hackbarth AD. Eliminating waste in US health care. Jama. 2012;307(14):1513–6. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.362 22419800

18. Delaune J, Everett W. Waste and inefficiency in the US health care system: clinical care: a comprehensive analysis in support of system-wide improvements: New England Healthcare Institute; 2008.

19. Yong PL, Saunders RS, Olsen L, editors. The healthcare imperative. The Healthcare Imperative: Lowering Costs and Improving Outcomes: Workshop Series Summary; 2010: National Academies Press (US).

20. Starfield B. Is US health really the best in the world? Jama. 2000;284(4):483–5. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.4.483 10904513

21. Schwartz LM, Woloshin S. Changing disease definitions: implications for disease prevalence. Analysis of the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1988–1994. Effective clinical practice: ECP. 1999;2(2):76–85. Epub 1999/10/28. 10538480.

22. Levinson W, Kallewaard M, Bhatia RS, Wolfson D, Shortt S, Kerr EA, et al. ‘Choosing Wisely’: a growing international campaign. BMJ quality & safety. 2014:bmjqs-2014-003821.

23. Colla CH, Kinsella EA, Morden NE, Meyers DJ, Rosenthal MB, Sequist TD. Physician perceptions of Choosing Wisely and drivers of overuse. The American journal of managed care. 2016;22(5):337–43. Epub 2016/06/09. 27266435.

24. Kost A, Genao I, Lee JW, Smith SR. Clinical decisions made in primary care clinics before and after Choosing Wisely™. The Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine. 2015;28(4):471–4. doi: 10.3122/jabfm.2015.05.140332 26152437

25. Kuehlein T, Sghedoni D, Visentin G, Guervas J, Jamoulle M. Quartäre Prävention–eine Aufgabe für Hausärzte. PrimaryCare. 2010;10(18):350–4.

26. Kuehlein T, Freund T, Joos S. Von der Kunst des Weglassens. Deutsches Ärzteblatt. 2013;110(48):2312–4.

27. Alber K, Kuehlein T, Schedlbauer A, Schaffer S. Medical overuse and quaternary prevention in primary care–A qualitative study with general practitioners. BMC family practice. 2017;18(1):99. doi: 10.1186/s12875-017-0667-4 29216841

28. von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gotzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP, et al. Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. BMJ (Clinical research ed). 2007;335(7624):806–8. Epub 2007/10/20. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39335.541782.AD 17947786.

29. Eysenbach G. Improving the quality of Web surveys: the Checklist for Reporting Results of Internet E-Surveys (CHERRIES). J Med Internet Res. 2004;6(3):e34. Epub 2004/10/09. doi: 10.2196/jmir.6.3.e34 15471760.

30. Mayring P, editor Combination and integration of qualitative and quantitative analysis. Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung/Forum: Qualitative Social Research; 2001.

31. Alber K, Kuehlein T, Schedlbauer A, Schaffer S. Medical overuse and quaternary prevention in primary care—A qualitative study with general practitioners. BMC family practice. 2017;18(1):99. Epub 2017/12/09. doi: 10.1186/s12875-017-0667-4 29216841.

32. Busfield J. Assessing the overuse of medicines. Social science & medicine (1982). 2015;131:199–206. Epub 2014/12/04. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.10.061 25464876.

33. Schwartz AL, Landon BE, Elshaug AG, Chernew ME, McWilliams JM. Measuring low-value care in Medicare. JAMA internal medicine. 2014;174(7):1067–76. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.1541 24819824

34. Communication PUR. Procedures In the Health Care System: What Physicians Say About The Problem, the Causes, and the Solutions. Results from a National Survey of Physicians, URL: http://www.choosingwisely.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/04/FinalChoosing-Wisely-Survey-Report.pdf. 2014.

35. Sirovich BE, Woloshin S, Schwartz LM. Too little? Too much? Primary care physicians’ views on US health care: a brief report. Archives of internal medicine. 2011;171(17):1582–5. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.437 21949169

36. Lyu H, Xu T, Brotman D, Mayer-Blackwell B, Cooper M, Daniel M, et al. Overtreatment in the United States. PLoS One. 2017;12(9):e0181970. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181970 28877170.

37. Gerbera M, Kraftb E, Bosshardc C. Overuse–unnötige Behandlungen als Qualitätsproblem.

38. Unnecessary Tests and Procedures In the Health Care System: What Physicians Say About The Problem, the Causes, and the Solutions. Results from a National Survey of Physicians. Choosing Wisely [cited 2017]. http://www.choosingwisely.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/04/Final-Choosing-Wisely-Survey-Report.pdf.

39. SurveyMonkey. [cited 2019]. http://www.surveymonkey.com

40. Nielsen J. Usability engineering. Boston: Academic press; 1993.

41. Choosing Wisely [cited 13.08.2019]. https://www.choosingwisely.org/.

42. Smarter Medicine [13.08.2019]. https://www.smartermedicine.ch/de/home.html.

43. Less is more [13.08.2019]. https://jamanetwork.com/collections/44045/less-is-more.

44. Klug entscheiden [13.08.2019]. https://www.klug-entscheiden.com/.

45. Lavrakas P. Encyclopedia of Survey Research Methods—Forced Choice. 2008. doi: 10.4135/9781412963947

46. Perreault WD. Controlling order-effect bias. The Public Opinion Quarterly. 1975;39(4):544–51.

47. Stocké V. Entstehungsbedingungen von Antwortverzerrungen durch soziale Erwünschtheit: Ein Vergleich der Prognosen der Rational-Choice Theorie und des Modells der Frame-Selektion/Determinants for Respondents’ Susceptibility to Social Desirability Bias: A Comparison of Predictions from Rational Choice Theory and the Model of Frame-Selection. Zeitschrift für Soziologie. 2004:303–20.

48. O’Sullivan ED, Schofield SJ. Cognitive bias in clinical medicine. J R Coll Physicians Edinb. 2018;48(3):225–32. Epub 2018/09/08. doi: 10.4997/JRCPE.2018.306 30191910.

49. Matthias M, Krebs EE, Bergman A, Coffing J, Bair M. Communicating about opioids for chronic pain: A qualitative study of patient attributions and the influence of the patient–physician relationship. European journal of pain. 2014;18(6):835–43. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00426.x 24921073

50. Buist DS, Chang E, Handley M, Pardee R, Gundersen G, Cheadle A, et al. Primary care clinicians’ perspectives on reducing low-value care in an integrated delivery system. The Permanente Journal. 2016;20(1):41. doi: 10.7812/TPP/15-086 26562308

51. Mayer FS, Duval S, Duval VH. An attributional analysis of commitment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1980;39(6):1072–80. doi: 10.1037/h0077726

52. Colla CH, Kinsella EA, Morden NE, Meyers DJ, Rosenthal MB, Sequist TD. Physician perceptions of Choosing Wisely and drivers of overuse. The American journal of managed care. 2016;22(5):337–43. 27266435


Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS One


2020 Číslo 1
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova

Svět praktické medicíny 1/2024 (znalostní test z časopisu)
nový kurz

Koncepce osteologické péče pro gynekology a praktické lékaře
Autoři: MUDr. František Šenk

Sekvenční léčba schizofrenie
Autoři: MUDr. Jana Hořínková

Hypertenze a hypercholesterolémie – synergický efekt léčby
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Hana Rosolová, DrSc.

Význam metforminu pro „udržitelnou“ terapii diabetu
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Milan Kvapil, CSc., MBA

Všechny kurzy
Kurzy Podcasty Doporučená témata Časopisy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#