#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Mushroom body subsets encode CREB2-dependent water-reward long-term memory in Drosophila


Authors: Wang-Pao Lee aff001;  Meng-Hsuan Chiang aff001;  Li-Yun Chang aff001;  Jhen-Yi Lee aff002;  Ya-Lun Tsai aff001;  Tai-Hsiang Chiu aff001;  Hsueh-Cheng Chiang aff003;  Tsai-Feng Fu aff004;  Tony Wu aff005;  Chia-Lin Wu aff001
Authors place of work: Graduate Institute of Biomedical Sciences, College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taiwan aff001;  School of Medicine, College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taiwan aff002;  Department of Pharmacology, National Cheng-Kung University, Taiwan aff003;  Department of Applied Chemistry, National Chi Nan University, Taiwan aff004;  Department of Neurology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taiwan aff005;  Department of Biochemistry, College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taiwan aff006
Published in the journal: Mushroom body subsets encode CREB2-dependent water-reward long-term memory in Drosophila. PLoS Genet 16(8): e32767. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1008963
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008963

Summary

Long-term memory (LTM) formation depends on the conversed cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB)-dependent gene transcription followed by de novo protein synthesis. Thirsty fruit flies can be trained to associate an odor with water reward to form water-reward LTM (wLTM), which can last for over 24 hours without a significant decline. The role of de novo protein synthesis and CREB-regulated gene expression changes in neural circuits that contribute to wLTM remains unclear. Here, we show that acute inhibition of protein synthesis in the mushroom body (MB) αβ or γ neurons during memory formation using a cold-sensitive ribosome-inactivating toxin disrupts wLTM. Furthermore, adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b in αβ or γ neurons also disrupts wLTM. The MB αβ and γ neurons can be further classified into five different neuronal subsets including αβ core, αβ surface, αβ posterior, γ main, and γ dorsal. We observed that the neurotransmission from αβ surface and γ dorsal neuron subsets is required for wLTM retrieval, whereas the αβ core, αβ posterior, and γ main are dispensable. Adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons inhibits wLTM formation. In vivo calcium imaging revealed that αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons form wLTM traces with different dynamic properties, and these memory traces are abolished by dCREB2b expression. Our results suggest that a small population of neurons within the MB circuits support long-term storage of water-reward memory in Drosophila.

Keywords:

Drosophila melanogaster – memory – Neurons – Conditioned response – Protein synthesis – Calcium imaging – neurotransmission – Olfactory receptor neurons

Introduction

Long-term memory (LTM) requires hours to transform labile memory to the long-lasting form of memory and needs de novo protein synthesis to support the synaptic morphology changes [1, 2, 3]. Moreover, in many species, LTM formation requires cAMP-response element-binding protein (CREB)-mediated gene transcription and protein synthesis [47]. CREB is a leucine-zipper transcription factor, which is evolutionarily conserved across species. The basic region of the leucine-zipper proteins binds to cAMP-response element (CRE) sites and regulates downstream gene expression. CREB knockout mice have been reported to have a defect in LTM although the initial learning and short-term memory (STM) is normal [8]. In Drosophila, both shock-punitive and sugar-reward LTMs require CREB2 activity, and ectopic expression of CREB2 repressor (dCREB2b) disrupts olfactory LTMs but not STMs [6, 9]. In Aplysia, microinjection of the CRE sequence into the nucleus of sensory neurons selectively blocks long-term facilitation without affecting the short-term facilitation [10].

Thirsty fruit flies can be trained to associate a specific odor (a conditioned stimulus, CS) with water (an unconditioned stimulus, US) to form water-reward memory, which can last for over 24 hours [1, 11]. De novo protein synthesis and several LTM specific genes are required for this process, suggesting that the long-lasting water memory is an LTM and not STM [1]. The dendritic regions of the mushroom body (MB), traditionally viewed as the olfactory memory center known as calyx, receive olfactory input from the antenna lobe via the projection neurons, and this olfactory information is transformed into sparse neural codes in the MB [12, 13]. The specific dopaminergic protocerebral anterior medial (PAM) neurons, called PAM-β′1 convey the water-rewarding inputs to the MB in thirsty flies [1]. Therefore, the association between an odor and water-reward is established in the MB, which is finally transformed into water-reward LTM (wLTM) [1].

The MB is a paired neuropil structure comprising approximately 2000 Kenyon cells in each brain hemisphere [14]. The Kenyon cells can be divided into αβ, γ, and α′β′ neurons according to their axonal distributions. Here, we report that genetic inactivation of protein synthesis by expressing the activated cold-sensitive ricin (RICINCS) during memory formation in αβ or γ neurons disrupts wLTM. Furthermore, adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b in αβ or γ neurons also disrupts wLTM. It has been shown that neurotransmission from αβ and γ neurons is required for wLTM retrieval [1]. The αβ and γ neurons can be further classified into five different subsets including αβ core, αβ surface, αβ posterior, γ main, and γ dorsal [15]. Our results showed that the neurotransmission from αβ surface and γ dorsal is required for wLTM retrieval, whereas the αβ core, αβ posterior, and γ main are dispensable. Adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b in αβ surface or γ dorsal also disrupts wLTM. Finally, we observed an increased in vivo calcium response in αβ surface and a decreased calcium response in γ dorsal in response to the training odor at 24-hour post-training in thirsty flies. These training-induced calcium response changes to odors were abolished by dCREB2b expression, suggesting that wLTM traces are formed in these MB circuits.

Results

De novo protein synthesis and CREB2 activity in αβ and γ neurons are critical for wLTM

It has been shown that both shock-punitive and sugar-reward LTMs require de novo protein synthesis in Drosophila [3, 9]. Our previous study also suggests that de novo protein synthesis is essential for wLTM formation [1]. To investigate whether protein synthesis in MB neurons is critical for wLTM formation, we genetically inactivated the protein synthesis in MB neurons by expressing RICINCS, a toxin that inhibits the eukaryotic ribosomes by cleaving the N-glycosidic bond of 28S rRNA [16, 17]. All the flies were kept at 18°C until eclosion to avoid the developmental effects of RICINCS and water-reward conditioning was performed at 18°C. The trained flies were then transferred to 30°C for 12 hours right after the training, and moved back to 18°C for another 12 hours and tested at 18°C. We observed that genetic inactivation of protein synthesis in MB neurons during memory formation disrupts wLTM (Fig 1A and S1 Fig). Furthermore, protein synthesis in MB γ and αβ neurons was found to be essential for wLTM formation, whereas it was dispensable in α′β′ neurons (Fig 1B–1G and S1 Fig). Since LTM formation is a CREB-dependent process that requires gene transcription and de novo protein synthesis, it prompted us to ask that CREB activity in which MB neurons are critical for wLTM. It has been shown that constitutive dCREB2b expression in MB neurons causes a significant neuroanatomical damage in the MB [18]. We therefore used tub-GAL80ts to limit UAS-dCREB2b expression to whole MB neurons (OK107-GAL4), γ neurons (R16A06-GAL4), αβ neurons (C739-GAL4), or α′β′ neurons (VT30604-GAL4) in adult flies to eliminate these developmental defects. Flies were kept at 18°C until eclosion to avoid dCREB2b expression during the developmental stage. After eclosion, adult flies were transferred to 30°C for dCREB2b transgene expression for three days. Water-reward conditioning was performed at 30°C and the trained flies were kept at 30°C for 8 hours, and then moved back to 18°C for 16 hours and tested. Flies in the control group were kept at 18°C throughout the experiment. We found that the adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b in γ or αβ neurons disrupted the wLTM, whereas dCREB2b expression in α′β′ neurons did not affect the wLTM (Fig 2 and S1 Fig).

Blocking <i>de novo</i> protein synthesis in αβ and γ neurons during memory formation disrupts wLTM.
Fig. 1. Blocking de novo protein synthesis in αβ and γ neurons during memory formation disrupts wLTM.
(A) Blocking protein synthesis in MB neurons using R13F02-GAL4 to drive the expression of activated RICINCS during memory formation impaired wLTM. After training at 18°C, the flies were moved to 30°C for 12 h to activate RICINCS, moved back to 18°C for another 12 h to inactivate RICINCS, and then tested for memory retention at 18°C (left panel). Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. The 24-hour water-reward memory was normal with inactive RICINCS (18°C) expression in MB neurons all the way during behavioral assay (right panel). Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA. (B-C) Blocking protein synthesis during memory formation in γ neurons using R16A06-GAL4 (B) or 5HT1B-GAL4 (C) to drive the expression of activated RICINCS (30°C, left panel) impaired wLTM. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. (D-E) Blocking protein synthesis in αβ neurons during memory formation using C739-GAL4 (D) or VT49246-GAL4 (E) to drive the expression of activated RICINCS (30°C, left panel) impaired wLTM. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. (F-G) Blocking protein synthesis in α′β′ neurons using VT30604-GAL4 (F) or VT57244-GAL4 (G) to drive the expression of activated RICINCS (30°C) did not affect wLTM. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA.
Adult stage-specific expression of <i>dCREB2b</i> in αβ and γ neurons disrupts wLTM.
Fig. 2. Adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b in αβ and γ neurons disrupts wLTM.
(A) Inducible expression of dCREB2b in MB neurons using OK107-GAL4 impaired the 24-hour water-reward memory. Adult flies were raised at 18°C and then transferred to 30°C for three days before training to remove tub-GAL80ts inhibition of GAL4 activity. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 7~9). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. (B) Inducible expression of dCREB2b in γ neurons using R16A06-GAL4 impaired the 24-hour water-reward memory. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 6~11). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. (C) Inducible expression of dCREB2b in αβ neurons using C739-GAL4 impaired the 24-hour water-reward memory. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. (D) Inducible expression of dCREB2b in α′β′ neurons using VT30604-GAL4 did not affect the 24-hour water-reward memory. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA.

wLTM retrieval requires neurotransmission from αβ surface and γ dorsal neuron subsets

The de novo protein synthesis and CREB2 activity in αβ and γ neuron subsets were found to be essential for wLTM formation (Figs 1 and 2). This led us to investigate whether the neurotransmission from specific αβ and γ neuron subsets plays a crucial role in wLTM. It has been shown that wLTM retrieval requires neurotransmission from αβ and γ neurons [1]. The αβ and γ neurons can be further classified into αβ core, αβ surface, αβ posterior, γ main, and γ dorsal subsets [15]. We analyzed the role of each αβ and γ neuron subsets in wLTM retrieval. The GAL4 labelled subsets of αβ and γ neurons were identified including αβ posterior (labeled by VT14429-GAL4 & VT24615-GAL4; Fig 3A and 3C), αβ core (labeled by VT0841-GAL4 & VT8347-GAL4; Fig 3E and 3G), αβ surface (labeled by VT20803-GAL4 & VT21845-GAL4; Fig 3I and 3K), γ main (labeled by R64C08-GAL4; Fig 4A), and γ dorsal (R93G04-GAL4 & MB607B-GAL4; Fig 4C and 4E). We genetically expressed the temperature-sensitive shibire (shits) transgene in individual αβ and γ neuron subset via these specific GAL4 lines. The transgenic flies were trained and kept at 23°C during the wLTM experiment and were moved to 32°C only during testing to block the neurotransmitter outputs in the memory retrieval phase. Blocking neurotransmission from αβ surface or γ dorsal neuron subsets during memory retrieval disrupted the wLTM (Figs 3J, 3L, 4D, 4F and S2 Fig), whereas blocking neurotransmission from αβ posterior (Fig 3B and 3D), αβ core (Fig 3F and 3H), or γ main neuron subsets (Fig 4B) during memory retrieval did not affect the wLTM.

Retrieval of wLTM requires neurotransmission from αβ surface neurons.
Fig. 3. Retrieval of wLTM requires neurotransmission from αβ surface neurons.
(A) The expression pattern of VT14429-GAL4 and the high-magnification signal from the horizontal confocal cross section of the MB vertical lobe (top right inset). (B) Blocking neurotransmission from αβ posterior neurons (VT14429-GAL4) using shits during retrieval did not affect the 24-hour water-reward memory. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA. (C) The expression pattern of VT24615-GAL4 and the high-magnification signal from the horizontal confocal cross section of the MB vertical lobe (top right inset). The brain neuropils were immunostained with anti-discs large (DLG) antibody (magenta). Scale bar represents 50 μm. (D) Blocking neurotransmission from αβ posterior neurons (VT24615-GAL4) using shits during retrieval did not affect the 24-hour water-reward memory. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA. (E) The expression pattern of VT0841-GAL4 and the high-magnification signal from the horizontal confocal cross section of the MB vertical lobe (top right inset). (F) Blocking neurotransmission from αβ core neurons (VT0841-GAL4) using shits during retrieval did not affect the 24-hour water-reward memory. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA. (G) The expression pattern of VT8347-GAL4 and the high-magnification signal from the horizontal confocal cross section of the MB vertical lobe (top right inset). The brain neuropils were immunostained with anti-DLG antibody (magenta). Scale bar represents 50 μm. (H) Blocking neurotransmission from αβ core neurons (VT8347-GAL4) using shits during retrieval did not affect the 24-hour water-reward memory. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA. (I) The expression pattern of VT20803-GAL4 and the high-magnification signal from the horizontal confocal cross section of the MB vertical lobe (top right inset). (J) Blocking neurotransmission from αβ surface neurons (VT20803-GAL4) using shits during retrieval disrupted the 24-hour water-reward memory (left panel). Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 7). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. The 24-hour water-reward memory was normal in VT20803-GAL4 > UAS-shits flies as compared to their internal control groups in 23°C (right panel). Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 6). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA. (K) The expression pattern of VT21845-GAL4 and the high-magnification signal from the horizontal confocal cross section of the MB vertical lobe (top right inset). The brain neuropils were immunostained with anti-DLG antibody (magenta). Scale bar represents 50 μm. (L) Blocking neurotransmission from αβ surface neurons (VT21845-GAL4) using shits during retrieval disrupted the 24-hour water-reward memory (left panel). Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. The 24-hour water-reward memory was normal in VT21845-GAL4 > UAS-shits flies as compared to their internal control groups in 23°C (right panel). Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA.
wLTM retrieval requires neurotransmission from γ dorsal neurons.
Fig. 4. wLTM retrieval requires neurotransmission from γ dorsal neurons.
(A) The expression pattern of R64C08-GAL4 and the high-magnification signal from the frontal confocal cross section of the MB horizontal lobe (top right inset). The brain neuropils were immunostained with anti-DLG antibody (magenta). Scale bar represents 50 μm. (B) Blocking neurotransmission from γ main neurons (R64C08-GAL4) using shits during retrieval did not affect wLTM. The 24-hour water-reward memory was normal in R64C08-GAL4 > UAS-shits flies as compared to their internal control groups in 32°C. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 10). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA. (C) The expression pattern of R93G04-GAL4 and the high-magnification signal from the frontal confocal cross section of the MB horizontal lobe (top right inset). (D) Blocking neurotransmission from γ dorsal neurons (R93G04-GAL4) using shits during retrieval disrupted wLTM (left panel). Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 12). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. The 24-hour water-reward memory was normal in R93G04-GAL4 > UAS-shits flies as compared to their internal control groups in 23°C (right panel). Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 6). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA. (E) The expression pattern of MB607B-GAL4 and the high-magnification signal from the frontal confocal cross section of the MB horizontal lobe (top right inset). The brain neuropils were immunostained with anti-DLG antibody (magenta). Scale bar represents 50 μm. (F) Blocking neurotransmission from γ dorsal neurons (MB607B-GAL4) using shits during retrieval disrupted wLTM (left panel). Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8~9). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. The 24-hour water-reward memory was normal in MB607B-GAL4 > UAS-shits flies as compared to their internal control groups in 23°C (right panel). Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 6). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA.

CREB2 activity in αβ surface and γ dorsal neuron subsets is critical for wLTM

Our results showed the CREB2 activity in αβ and γ neuron subsets is essential for wLTM formation (Fig 2). In addition, neurotransmission from αβ surface or γ dorsal neuron subsets is required for wLTM (Figs 3 and 4). These results together led us to investigate whether the CREB2 activity in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons is essential for wLTM. We, therefore, tested the effect of adult stage-specific dCREB2b expression in each αβ and γ neuron subset on wLTM. We used tub-GAL80ts to limit UAS-dCREB2b expression to αβ surface (VT20803-GAL4), γ dorsal (R93G04-GAL4), αβ posterior (VT24615-GAL4), αβ core (VT8347-GAL4), or γ main (R64C08-GAL4) in adult flies to eliminate the developmental defects of MB neurons. We found that adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b in αβ surface or γ dorsal neuron subsets disrupted the wLTM (Fig 5A and 5B). However, adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b in αβ core, αβ posterior, or γ main neuron subsets did not affect wLTM formation (Fig 5C–5E). In addition, we found that blocking de novo protein synthesis in αβ surface or γ dorsal neuron subsets also disrupted the wLTM (S3 Fig). Taken together, these results suggest that wLTM formation requires CREB2 activity and de novo protein synthesis specifically in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons.

Adult stage-specific expression of <i>dCREB2b</i> in αβ surface or γ dorsal neurons disrupts wLTM.
Fig. 5. Adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b in αβ surface or γ dorsal neurons disrupts wLTM.
(A) Inducible expression of dCERB2b in αβ surface neurons (VT20803-GAL4) disrupted wLTM. The 24-hour water-reward memory was disrupted in flies carrying VT20803-GAL4 > UAS-dCREB2b; tub-GAL80ts compared to their internal controls. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. (B) Inducible expression of dCERB2b in γ dorsal neurons disrupted wLTM. The 24-hour water-reward memory was disrupted in flies carrying R93G04-GAL4 > UAS-dCREB2b; tub-GAL80ts compared to their internal controls. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8~10). *, p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. (C) Inducible expression of dCERB2b in αβ posterior (VT24615-GAL4) did not affect wLTM. The 24-hour water-reward memory was normal in flies carrying VT24615-GAL4 > UAS-dCREB2b; tub-GAL80ts compared to their internal controls. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8~10). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA. (D) Inducible expression of dCERB2b in αβ core (VT8347-GAL4) neurons did not affect wLTM. The 24-hour water-reward memory was normal in flies carrying VT8347-GAL4 > UAS-dCREB2b; tub-GAL80ts compared to their internal controls. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8~10). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA. (E) Inducible expression of dCERB2b in γ main (R64C08-GAL4) neurons did not affect wLTM. The 24-hour water-reward memory was normal in flies carrying R64C08-GAL4 > UAS-dCREB2b; tub-GAL80ts compared to their internal controls. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8~10). p > 0.05; one-way ANOVA.

Different dynamics of cellular calcium response to training odor in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons

LTM formation occurs through a series of changes within neurons, which is essential to encode the relevant sensory inputs. The cellular calcium response to the associative stimuli can be any neural activity change induced by learning, which alters the neuronal response to the sensory information. This change allows the neurons to become more or less excitable, which in turn makes them more or less capable to fire an action potential. Functional in vivo calcium imaging has provided insights into neuronal responses to specific odors in a living fly brain by allowing the examination of increased or decreased calcium responses to training odor after conditioning [19]. Several previous studies have shown that different calcium responses to training odor (called memory trace) occurs in the distinct axons of MB neurons at different time windows after odor/shock conditioning [2023]. We therefore investigated whether the thirsty flies could form a memory trace in MB neurons after odor/water conditioning, and the paired and unpaired training protocols were used for in vivo calcium imaging analysis. For the paired training group, the thirsty flies carrying UAS-GCaMP6m plus VT20803-GAL4 received CS− odor without water, followed by exposure to the CS+ odor with water. For the unpaired training group, the thirsty flies received CS− odor without water, followed by exposure to the CS+ odor without water, and the water was provided 1-minute after CS+ odor delivery. A significantly increased calcium response to training odor was observed in the α-lobe region of the αβ surface neurons, but not in the αβ core neurons at 24-hour after odor/water association in thirsty flies (Fig 6 and S4 Fig). The increased cellular calcium response to training odor was eliminated in the same flies carrying UAS-dCREB2b transgene, suggesting that this 24-hour memory trace is CREB2-dependent (Fig 6). Moreover, the increased cellular calcium response to training odor occurred not only in the α-lobe but also in the β-lobe of surface neurons (S5 Fig).

αβ surface neurons show a CREB2-dependent increased cellular calcium response to training odor at 24-hour post-conditioning.
Fig. 6. αβ surface neurons show a CREB2-dependent increased cellular calcium response to training odor at 24-hour post-conditioning.
Top diagram illustrating the protocols of paired training, unpaired training, and in vivo calcium imaging. (A) The GCaMP6 response 24-hour after water-reward conditioning was assayed in αβ surface neurons (the image-recording region is showed in the top left figure). For the paired training group: flies received CS− odor without water-reward (US), followed by exposure to the CS+ odor with water-reward. For the unpaired training group: flies received CS− odor without water-reward, followed by exposure to CS+ odor without water-reward, and water-reward was delivered 1-minute after CS+ odor. Odor/water paired training induced additional increment of GCaMP6 responses in the α-lobe region of αβ surface neurons to the training odor [OCT-trained flies: OCT (CS+), MCH-trained flies: MCH (CS+)] in thirsty-state. This additional increased calcium response was abolished in water-sated state (flies allowed to drink water for 30 minutes before in vivo calcium imaging recording) or in thirsty flies with dCREB2b expression in αβ surface neurons. (B) Quantification of the GCaMP6 responses to the training odor (CS+) relative to the non-training odor (CS−) in the α-lobe region of αβ surface neurons in OCT-trained (left panel) or MCH-trained (right panel) flies at 24-hour post-conditioning. The Log ratios of the CS+ response to the CS− response were calculated using the peak response amplitudes. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 8~12). *, p < 0.05; significantly different from zero; one sample t-test. Genotype: (1) Flies carrying UAS-GCaMP6m/tub-GAL80ts;VT20803-GAL4/+ transgenes were used for Paired training (thirsty), Paired training (water sated), and Unpaired training (thirsty) experiments, (2) Flies carrying UAS-GCaMP6m/tub-GAL80ts; VT20803-GAL4/UAS-dCREB2b transgenes were used for Paired training (dCREB2b; thirsty) experiment.

In addition, thirsty flies carrying UAS-GCaMP6m plus R93G04-GAL4 were trained to associate an odor with water, and 24-hour odor responses were recorded. Intriguingly, we found that naïve flies showed decreased calcium responses to odor in the γ dorsal neurons (Fig 7 and S6 Fig). However, in the paired training group, the decrease in calcium response to training odor was even stronger compared to the naïve flies 24-hour after training (Fig 7). This training-induced additional decrease in calcium response to training odor was eliminated in the same flies carrying UAS-dCREB2b transgene. These results suggest that thirsty flies show an increased cellular calcium response to training odor in αβ surface neurons and a decreased cellular calcium response to training odor in γ dorsal neurons at 24-hour after odor/water conditioning, which implies that different neuronal dynamics of wLTM trace occur in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons (Fig 8).

γ dorsal neurons show a CREB2-dependent decreased cellular calcium response to training odor at 24-hour post-conditioning.
Fig. 7. γ dorsal neurons show a CREB2-dependent decreased cellular calcium response to training odor at 24-hour post-conditioning.
Top diagram illustrating the protocols of paired training, unpaired training, and in vivo calcium imaging. (A) The GCaMP6 response 24-hour after water-reward conditioning was assayed in γ dorsal neurons (the image-recording region is showed in the top left figure). For the paired training group: flies received CS− odor without water-reward (US), followed by exposure to the CS+ odor with water-reward. For the unpaired training group: flies received CS− odor without water-reward, followed by exposure to CS+ odor without water-reward, and the water-reward was delivered 1-minute after CS+ odor. Odor/water paired training induced an additional decrease in GCaMP6 responses in the γ-lobe region of the γ dorsal neurons to the training odor [OCT-trained flies: OCT (CS+), MCH-trained flies: MCH (CS+)] in thirsty-state. This additional decreased calcium response was abolished in water-sated state (flies allowed to drink water for 30 minutes before in vivo calcium imaging recording) or in thirsty flies with dCREB2b expression in γ dorsal neurons. (B) Quantification of the GCaMP6 responses to the training odor (CS+) relative to the non-training odor (CS−) in the γ-lobe region of γ dorsal neurons in OCT-trained (left panel) or MCH-trained (right panel) flies at 24-hour post-conditioning. The Log ratios of the CS+ response to the CS− response were calculated using the peak response amplitudes. Each value represents mean ± SEM (N = 9~11). *, p < 0.05; statistically significantly different from zero; one sample t-test. Genotype: (1) Flies carrying UAS-GCaMP6m/tub-GAL80ts; R93G04-GAL4/+ transgenes were used for Paired training (thirsty), Paired training (water sated), and Unpaired training (thirsty) experiments, (2) Flies carrying UAS-GCaMP6m/tub-GAL80ts; R93G04-GAL4/UAS-dCREB2b transgenes were used for Paired training (dCREB2b; thirsty) experiment.
A brain circuitry model of <i>CREB2</i> and wLTM.
Fig. 8. A brain circuitry model of CREB2 and wLTM.
The αβ surface neurons show an evoked calcium response to odors while the γ dorsal neurons show a decreased calcium response to odors in naïve flies. After odor/water association which induces the formation of wLTM, calcium response to training odor was further increased in αβ surface neurons, whereas the calcium response was further decreased in the γ dorsal neurons 24-hour after training. These increased and decreased cellular calcium responses to training odor in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons, respectively, were abolished by dCREB2b expression, suggesting that training-induced cellular calcium responsive changes in distinct MB subsets form wLTM traces with different neuronal dynamics in thirsty fly.

Discussion

CREB-dependent gene transcription is critical for memory formation, especially LTM, in both vertebrates and invertebrates [4, 24]. Several previous studies in Drosophila suggest that LTM formation requires CREB-dependent gene transcription followed by de novo protein synthesis [1, 3, 6, 9]. Moreover, it has been reported that CREB2 activity in both αβ and α′β′ neurons is critical for appetitive LTM produced by sugar-reward conditioning [9, 25]. Here, we observed that wLTM formation requires de novo protein synthesis in the αβ and γ neurons (Fig 1). Moreover, adult stage-specific expression of the CREB2 repressor (dCREB2b) in αβ or γ neurons disrupts wLTM, whereas CREB2 activity in α′β′ neurons is dispensable (Fig 2). Food or water deprivation is necessary to induce the motivational drive in flies to form sugar- or water-reward LTM since different motivational drives are critical for distinct memories. It has been shown that individual internal motivational inputs for sugar and water are delivered via distinct MB input neurons, which finally induce sugar- or water-reward LTM in different MB neuron subsets [1, 9, 26, 27]. Previous studies together with our current findings suggest that CREB2 activity is required for both sugar- and water-reward LTMs, however, these LTMs are processed in different MB circuits [9, 25].

Neurotransmission from αβ neurons is required for both shock-punitive and sugar-reward LTMs retrieval [28], whereas neurotransmission from γ neurons is dispensable for retrieval of both shock-punitive and sugar-reward LTMs [28, 29]. However, our previous study showed that the neurotransmission from αβ and γ neurons is required for wLTM retrieval [1] suggesting that wLTM is different from the other types of olfactory associative LTMs at MB circuit levels. The αβ and γ neurons are classified into αβ core, αβ surface, αβ posterior, γ main, and γ dorsal neuron subsets according to the morphology of their axons [15]. It has been shown that neurotransmission from the combination of MB αβ surface and αβ posterior subsets is necessary for the retrieval of both shock-punitive and sugar-rewarded LTMs [30]. Another study suggests that the neurotransmission from αβ surface and αβ core subsets is required for the retrieval of sugar-reward LTM [31]. These results imply that several different subsets of αβ neurons participate in the retrieval of Drosophila sugar-reward LTM. Here, we showed that neurotransmission only from αβ surface is necessary for wLTM retrieval, whereas the αβ core and αβ posterior subdivisions are dispensable (Fig 3 and S2 Fig). Contrary to the sugar reward conditioning in which γ neurons are dispensable for the retrieval of sugar-reward LTM [28, 29], wLTM retrieval requires neurotransmission from γ dorsal but not from γ main neuron subset (Fig 4 and S2 Fig). Taken together, these results imply that γ dorsal neuronal activity is specifically required for wLTM retrieval but not for sugar-reward LTM. Adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b or blocking de novo protein synthesis in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons disrupts wLTM, further suggesting the crucial role of αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons in Drosophila wLTM process (Fig 5 and S3 Fig).

Our previous study suggests that PAM-β′1 neurons convey the water-rewarding event as the US signal to the MB β′ lobes, and the neurotransmission in α′β′ neurons is required for wLTM consolidation [1]. How the wLTM is transferred from α′β′ neurons and finally stored in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons through system consolidation is still unclear. In both shock-punitive and sugar-reward LTMs, the neurotransmission in α′β′, γ, and, αβ neurons is required for at least 3 hours after conditioning, but the expression of 24-hour shock-punitive or sugar-rewarded memories only requires neurotransmission in αβ neurons [28]. In addition, the expression of DopR1, a D1-like dopamine receptor, in the γ neurons is sufficient to fully support the shock-punitive STM and LTM in DopR1 mutant background (dumb2), suggesting that dopamine-mediated odor/shock association is registered in γ neurons and finally stabilized and maintained in αβ neurons for long-term storage [32]. It might be possible that other MB input neurons (i.e., PAM) convey water rewarding events to MB αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons individually during water drinking in thirsty flies, which allow the initial odor/water associations in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons [1, 11]. The synaptic output from α′β′ neurons is required for stabilizing and strengthening the associative plasticity in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons during wLTM consolidation. How the α′β′ neurons communicate with αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons in wLTM consolidation phase, remains unclear. The intrinsic MB neurons, also known as the dorsal paired medial (DPM), arborize widely throughout the MB lobes and play a critical role in modulating MB functions [3338], including the consolidation of olfactory memories [9, 39, 40]. It might also be possible that DPM neurons receive the input from α′β′ neurons and transmit to αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons during wLTM consolidation. In addition, the output of MB Kenyon cells is conveyed to 34 neurons (MB output neuron, MBON) of 21 cell types per brain hemisphere, and 9 MBONs receive the inputs from α′β′ neurons [41]. Another possibility is that the activity from α′β′ neurons is transmitted to αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons via the relevant α′β′ MBONs and their downstream neurons. Therefore, it is noteworthy to test the physiological roles of DPM neurons and α′β′ MBONs during consolidation phase of wLTM.

A significant increase in cellular calcium response to training odor in the αβ surface neurons, but not in other αβ neuron subsets, was observed 24-hour after water-reward conditioning (Fig 6, S4 Fig and S5 Fig). These results are consistent with our behavioral study showing that neurotransmission from αβ surface neurons is required for wLTM retrieval, whereas the αβ core and αβ posterior neuron subsets are dispensable (Fig 3). This training-induced increased calcium response was abolished in water-sated or dCREB2b expressing flies (Fig 6). A previous study showed that the fly forms α-lobe branch-specific aversive LTM trace 24-hour after odor/shock conditioning [20]. In our recent study, we also observed the α-lobe branch-specific aversive anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM) trace 3-hour after odor/shock conditioning [23]. Intriguingly, we found that both α- and β- lobes of the surface neurons show increased calcium response to training odor 24-hour after odor/water conditioning, suggesting that the wLTM trace is not specific to the α-lobe branch (Fig 6 and S5 Fig).

An increased GCaMP response to training odor in MB γ neurons is observed at 24-hour after ten sessions of spaced odor/shock training, and this increased calcium response is abolished by expressing dCREB2b in γ neurons throughout the fly development [21]. Here, we found that γ main neuron subset shows an evoked calcium response to both odors, but no further increased calcium response to the training odor at 24-hour after water-reward conditioning was observed (S7 Fig). A previous study suggests that γ dorsal neurons respond to visual stimuli, which is required for visual, but not for aversive olfactory memory in Drosophila [42]. However, we observed a decreased calcium response to odor stimuli in the γ dorsal lobe (Fig 7 and S6 Fig), which is consistent with the electrophysiological study showing slow inhibitory responses to odor stimuli in the γ dorsal neurons [42]. Interestingly, we observed a further decrease in calcium responses to the training odor in the γ dorsal neurons 24-hour after water-reward conditioning (Fig 7). This training-induced additional decrease in the calcium response is abolished in the water-sated or acutely dCREB2b expressing flies, suggesting a type of wLTM trace in the γ dorsal neurons different from the αβ surface neurons (Figs 6, 7 and 8). Since blocking neurotransmission from the γ dorsal neuron subset during memory retrieval disrupts wLTM, why the γ dorsal neurons show additionally suppressed calcium response to training odor, needs to be answered. One possible explanation is that odor/water association alters the olfactory response of MB neurons to the training odor, and this change can be represented by an increased or decreased calcium response as compared to the response of the non-training odor (memory traces). The training-induced differences in odor responsive levels in the MB allow the flies to distinguish two odors by increasing the contrast and perform appropriate behavioral output during testing. However, shits abolishes the increased or decreased training-odor responses thereby eliminating the contrast between odors, and consequently, the flies could not distinguish two odors and make appropriate behavioral output during testing.

In conclusion, our study show that αβ surface and γ dorsal neuron subsets regulate Drosophila wLTM. Blocking neurotransmission from αβ surface or γ dorsal neurons only abolishes wLTM retrieval but does not affect the olfactory acuity or water preference in thirsty flies (Figs 3, 4 and S2 Fig). Further, adult stage-specific expression of dCREB2b or blocking de novo protein synthesis in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons disrupts wLTM (Fig 5 and S3 Fig). Different dynamics of cellular wLTM traces are formed in the αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons, which are blocked by dCREB2b expression (Figs 6, 7 and 8). Taken together, our results reveal a small population of MB neurons that encode wLTM in the brain and provide a broader view of the olfactory memory process in fruit flies.

Materials and methods

Fly stocks

All the flies were reared on standard cornmeal food at 25°C and 60% relative humidity on a 12 h:12 h, light-dark cycle. R13F02-GAL4, R16A06-GAL4, 5HT1B-GAL4, C739-GAL4, VT44966-GAL4, VT30604-GAL4, VT57244-GAL4, tub-GAL80ts; UAS-dCREB2b, UAS-mCD8::GFP; UAS-mCD8::GFP, UAS-shits, and UAS-GCaMP6m fly strains have been used as described previously [1, 23, 35, 37, 4345]. UAS-ricinCS and MB607B-GAL4 flies were obtained from Ann-Shyn Chiang. OK107-GAL4, VT14429-GAL4, VT24615-GAL4, VT20803-GAL4, VT21845-GAL4, VT0841-GAL4, VT8347-GAL4, R64C08-GAL4, and R93G04-GAL4 fly strains were obtained from the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center and Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center.

Immunohistochemistry and brain imaging

Fly brains were dissected in isotonic phosphate buffered saline (PBS) and transferred to 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) for fixation for 20 min at 25°C. The samples were then incubated in penetration and blocking buffer (PBS containing 2% Triton X-100 and 10% normal goat serum) for 2 h at 25°C. The brain samples were also subjected to a degassing procedure during the 2 h penetration and blocking period. Thereafter, the brains were incubated in 1:10 diluted mouse 4F3 anti-discs large (DLG) monoclonal antibody (AB 528203, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, University of Iowa) at 25°C for 24 h. After washing in PBS-T (PBS containing 1% Triton X-100), the samples were incubated in 1:200 biotinylated goat anti-mouse IgG (31800, Thermo Fisher Scientific) at 25°C for 24 h. Next, the brain samples were washed and incubated in 1:500 Alexa Fluor 635 streptavidin (S32364, Thermo Fisher Scientific) at 25°C for 24 h. After intensive washing in PBS-T, the brains were cleared and mounted in FocusClear (FC-101, CelExplorer) for imaging. The brains were imaged under a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal microscope with either a 40× C-Apochromat water-immersion objective (N.A. value, 1.2; working distance, 220 μm) or a 63× glycerin-immersion objective (N.A. value, 1.4; working distance, 170 μm). The confocal pinhole (optical section) was set at 2 μm for images taken with the 40× objective lens and at 1.5 μm when imaging at 63× objective lens. Some slides were imaged twice to overcome the limited field of view; one image for each brain hemisphere, with an overlap in between. Two parallel brain image stacks were combined into a single dataset using the overlapping region to align the two confocal stacks by ZEN software.

Behavioral assay

The wLTM assay has been described in our previous study [1]. Briefly, the flies were water deprived by keeping them in a glass milk bottle containing a 6 cm × 3 cm piece of dry sucrose-soaked filter paper for 16 h before water-reward conditioning assay. The flies were first exposed to one odor for 2 min (CS−: 4-methylcyclohexanol (MCH) or 3-octanol (OCT)) in a tube lined with dry filter paper, followed by 1 min of fresh room air. The flies were then transferred to another tube that contained a water-soaked filter paper and exposed to a second odor for another 2 min (CS+: OCT or MCH). After that, the flies were transferred to a clean training tube and exposed to fresh room air for 1 min. The trained flies were kept in a plastic vial that contained a 1.5 cm × 3 cm piece of a dried sucrose-soaked filter paper during the 24 h interval. During the testing phase, the flies were presented with a choice between CS+ and CS− odors in a T-maze for 2 min. From this distribution, a performance index (PI) was calculated as the number of flies running toward the CS+ odor minus the number of flies running towards the CS− odor, divided by the total number of flies and multiplied by 100. For the calculation of individual PI, naive flies were first trained by pairing water with OCT (CS+), and the PIO index was calculated. Next, another group of naive flies was trained by pairing water with MCH (CS+), and the PIM index was calculated. A single PI was calculated from the average of single PIO and PIM values. For the RICINCS-related experiments, the flies were kept at 18°C until eclosion and during the training. The trained flies were then moved to 30°C for 12 h right after the training, and moved back to 18°C for another 12 h and then tested. For the control groups, the flies were kept at 18°C throughout the whole experiment. In shits-related experiments for blocking neurotransmitter output during wLTM retrieval, flies were trained at 23°C and then maintained at 23°C for 23.5 h followed by 32°C for 0.5 h and tested. For adult stage-specific dCREB2b expression with tub-GAL80ts, flies were kept at 18°C until eclosion and then moved to 30°C for 3 days before water conditioning. The water-reward conditioning was performed at 30°C and the trained flies were kept at 30°C for 8 h and then moved back to 18°C for 16 h and tested. For the control groups, flies were kept at 18°C throughout the whole experiment.

Olfactory acuity assay

Groups of approximately 50 water-deprived naïve flies were subjected to a 2-min test trial in the T-maze at a restrictive temperature (32°C) for olfactory acuity assay. Flies were given a choice between OCT/MCH and ‘fresh’ room air. The odor avoidance index was calculated as the number of flies in the fresh room-air tube minus the number of flies in the OCT or MCH odor tube, divided by the total number of flies, and multiplied by 100.

Water preference assay

Groups of approximately 50 water-deprived naïve flies were given 2 min to choose between tubes in a T-maze at a restrictive temperature (32°C) for water preference assay, with one tube containing a dry filter paper and the other tube containing a water-soaked filter paper. The water preference index was calculated as the number of flies in the tube containing a water-soaked filter paper minus the number of flies in the tube containing a dry filter paper, divided by the total number of flies, and multiplied by 100.

In vivo calcium imaging

The flies carrying (1) UAS-GCaMP6m/tub-GAL80ts; VT20803-GAL4/+, (2) UAS-GCaMP6m/tub-GAL80ts; VT20803-GAL4/UAS-dCREB2b, (3) UAS-GCaMP6m/tub-GAL80ts; R93G04-GAL4/+, and (4) UAS-GCaMP6m/tub-GAL80ts; R93G04-GAL4/UAS-dCREB2b transgenes were kept at 18°C until eclosion. After, the flies were kept at 30°C for 3 days before training. The water-reward conditioning was performed at 30°C and the trained flies were kept at 30°C for 8 h then moved back to 18°C for another 16 h. The in vivo calcium imaging was performed at 25°C. The flies carrying (1) UAS-GCaMP6m/+; VT0841-GAL4/+, (2) UAS-GCaMP6m/+; VT20803-GAL4/+, (3) UAS-GCaMP6m/+; R93G04-GAL4/+, and (4) UAS-GCaMP6m/+; R64C08-GAL4/+ transgenes were kept at 25°C throughout the experiment. For the paired training group: flies received CS− odor without water, followed by exposure to the CS+ odor with water. For the unpaired training group: flies received CS− odor without water, followed by exposure to the CS+ odor without water, and water was provided 1 min after the CS+ odor delivery.

The flies from the paired or unpaired training groups were immobilized in a 250-ml pipette tip. A window was opened on the head capsule using fine tweezers and a drop of adult hemolymph-like (AHL) saline (108 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 2 mM CaCl2, 8.2 mM MgCl2, 4 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 5 mM trehalose, 10 mM sucrose, and 5 mM HEPES (pH 7.5, 265 mOsm)) was added immediately to prevent dehydration. After removing the small trachea and excessive fat with fine tweezers, the fly and the pipette tip were fixed to a coverslip by tape, and a 40× water immersion objective (W Plan-Apochromat 40×/1.0 DIC M27) was used for imaging. Odorants were delivered via a custom-made odor delivery device. The frames were aligned using a lightweight SIFT-implementation to correct the motion artifacts. The calcium responses were calculated as the mean change in fluorescence intensity (ΔF/F) in the 0.1–5 s window after stimulus onset. Time-lapse recordings of changes in GCaMP intensity before and after odor delivery were performed under a Zeiss LSM700 microscope with an excitation laser (488 nm) and a detector for emissions passing through a 555 nm short-pass filter. An optical slice with a resolution of 512 × 512 pixels was continuously monitored for 45 s at a rate of 2 frames per second. Regions of interest were manually assigned to anatomically different regions of the MB lobes. To evaluate responses to different odors in flies, we calculated the changes in GCaMP6 fluorescence as ΔF(Ft–F0)/F(F0). Changes in GCaMP6 fluorescent intensity for the CS+ vs. CS− odors were calculated as log10 (ΔFCS+/ΔFCS−). The intensity maps were generated using ImageJ software for all functional calcium imaging studies.

Statistical analysis

All the raw data were analyzed parametrically using the Prism 7.0 software (GraphPad). The raw data from more than two groups were evaluated by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Tukey’s multiple comparison test. The raw data from only two groups were evaluated by paired t-test. The one sample t-test was used to evaluate whether the raw data from one group is significantly different from zero or not. A statistically significant difference was defined as P < 0.05. All data were presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM).

Supporting information

S1 Fig [a]
GFP expression pattern driven by MB-GAL4s used in this study.

S2 Fig [a]
Behavioral control experiments in MB-GAL4s combined with .

S3 Fig [a]
Blocking protein synthesis in αβ surface and γ dorsal neurons during memory formation disrupts wLTM.

S4 Fig [a]
αβ core neurons do not form wLTM trace.

S5 Fig [a]
Horizontal lobe of αβ surface neurons forms wLTM trace.

S6 Fig [a]
γ dorsal neurons show decreased calcium responses to odors.

S7 Fig [a]
γ main neurons do not form wLTM trace.

S1 Table [xlsx]
Numerical data for graphs.


Zdroje

1. Shyu WH, Chiu TH, Chiang MH, Cheng YC, Tsai YL, Fu TF, et al. Neural circuits for long-term water-reward memory processing in thirsty Drosophila. Nat Commun. 2017;8:15230. Epub 2017/05/16. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15230 28504254; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5440665.

2. Davis HP, Squire LR. Protein synthesis and memory: a review. Psychol Bull. 1984;96(3):518–59. Epub 1984/11/01. 6096908.

3. Tully T, Preat T, Boynton SC, Del Vecchio M. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila. Cell. 1994;79(1):35–47. Epub 1994/10/07. 0092-8674(94)90398-0 [pii]. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90398-0 7923375.

4. Yin JCP, Tully T. CREB and the formation of long-term memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1996;6(2):264–8. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80082-1 WOS:A1996UN15100016. 8725970

5. Kandel ER, Dudai Y, Mayford MR. The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell. 2014;157(1):163–86. Epub 2014/04/01. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.001 24679534.

6. Yin JC, Wallach JS, Del Vecchio M, Wilder EL, Zhou H, Quinn WG, et al. Induction of a dominant negative CREB transgene specifically blocks long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell. 1994;79(1):49–58. Epub 1994/10/07. 0092-8674(94)90399-9 [pii]. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90399-9 7923376.

7. Perazzona B, Isabel G, Preat T, Davis RL. The role of cAMP response element-binding protein in Drosophila long-term memory. J Neurosci. 2004;24(40):8823–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4542-03.2004 15470148.

8. Bourtchuladze R, Frenguelli B, Blendy J, Cioffi D, Schutz G, Silva AJ. Deficient long-term memory in mice with a targeted mutation of the cAMP-responsive element-binding protein. Cell. 1994;79(1):59–68. Epub 1994/10/07. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90400-6 7923378.

9. Krashes MJ, Waddell S. Rapid consolidation to a radish and protein synthesis-dependent long-term memory after single-session appetitive olfactory conditioning in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 2008;28(12):3103–13. Epub 2008/03/21. 28/12/3103 [pii] doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5333-07.2008 18354013; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2516741.

10. Dash PK, Hochner B, Kandel ER. Injection of the cAMP-responsive element into the nucleus of Aplysia sensory neurons blocks long-term facilitation. Nature. 1990;345(6277):718–21. Epub 1990/06/21. doi: 10.1038/345718a0 2141668.

11. Lin S, Owald D, Chandra V, Talbot C, Huetteroth W, Waddell S. Neural correlates of water reward in thirsty Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(11):1536–42. Epub 2014/09/30. doi: 10.1038/nn.3827 25262493; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4213141.

12. Honegger KS, Campbell RA, Turner GC. Cellular-resolution population imaging reveals robust sparse coding in the Drosophila mushroom body. J Neurosci. 2011;31(33):11772–85. Epub 2011/08/19. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1099-11.2011 21849538; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3180869.

13. Turner GC, Bazhenov M, Laurent G. Olfactory representations by Drosophila mushroom body neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99(2):734–46. doi: 10.1152/jn.01283.2007 18094099

14. Aso Y, Grubel K, Busch S, Friedrich AB, Siwanowicz I, Tanimoto H. The mushroom body of adult Drosophila characterized by GAL4 drivers. J Neurogenet. 2009;23(1–2):156–72. Epub 2009/01/14. doi: 10.1080/01677060802471718 19140035.

15. Aso Y, Hattori D, Yu Y, Johnston RM, Iyer NA, Ngo TT, et al. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. Elife. 2014;3. Epub 2014/12/24. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04577 25535793.

16. Endo Y, Mitsui K, Motizuki M, Tsurugi K. The mechanism of action of ricin and related toxic lectins on eukaryotic ribosomes. The site and the characteristics of the modification in 28 S ribosomal RNA caused by the toxins. J Biol Chem. 1987;262(12):5908–12. Epub 1987/04/25. 3571242.

17. Endo Y, Tsurugi K. RNA N-glycosidase activity of ricin A-chain. Mechanism of action of the toxic lectin ricin on eukaryotic ribosomes. J Biol Chem. 1987;262(17):8128–30. Epub 1987/06/15. 3036799.

18. Chen CC, Wu JK, Lin HW, Pai TP, Fu TF, Wu CL, et al. Visualizing long-term memory formation in two neurons of the Drosophila brain. Science. 2012;335(6069):678–85. Epub 2012/02/11. doi: 10.1126/science.1212735 22323813.

19. Davis RL. Traces of Drosophila memory. Neuron. 2011;70(1):8–19. Epub 2011/04/13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.012 21482352.

20. Yu D, Akalal DB, Davis RL. Drosophila alpha/beta mushroom body neurons form a branch-specific, long-term cellular memory trace after spaced olfactory conditioning. Neuron. 2006;52(5):845–55. Epub 2006/12/06. S0896-6273(06)00830-0 [pii] doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.030 17145505; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1779901.

21. Akalal DB, Yu D, Davis RL. A late-phase, long-term memory trace forms in the gamma neurons of Drosophila mushroom bodies after olfactory classical conditioning. J Neurosci. 2010;30(49):16699–708. Epub 2010/12/15. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1882-10.2010 21148009; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3380342.

22. Wang Y, Mamiya A, Chiang AS, Zhong Y. Imaging of an early memory trace in the Drosophila mushroom body. J Neurosci. 2008;28(17):4368–76. Epub 2008/04/25. 28/17/4368 [pii] doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2958-07.2008 18434515.

23. Shyu WH, Lee WP, Chiang MH, Chang CC, Fu TF, Chiang HC, et al. Electrical synapses between mushroom body neurons are critical for consolidated memory retrieval in Drosophila. PLoS Genet. 2019;15(5):e1008153. Epub 2019/05/10. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1008153 31071084; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6529013.

24. Silva AJ, Kogan JH, Frankland PW, Kida S. CREB and memory. Ann Rev Neurosci. 1998;21:127–48. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.21.1.127 WOS:000072446400006. 9530494

25. Widmer YF, Fritsch C, Jungo MM, Almeida S, Egger B, Sprecher SG. Multiple neurons encode CrebB dependent appetitive long-term memory in the mushroom body circuit. Elife. 2018;7. Epub 2018/10/23. doi: 10.7554/eLife.39196 30346271; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6234028.

26. Senapati B, Tsao CH, Juan YA, Chiu TH, Wu CL, Waddell S, et al. A neural mechanism for deprivation state-specific expression of relevant memories in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22(12):2029–39. Epub 2019/10/30. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0515-z 31659341.

27. Krashes MJ, DasGupta S, Vreede A, White B, Armstrong JD, Waddell S. A neural circuit mechanism integrating motivational state with memory expression in Drosophila. Cell. 2009;139(2):416–27. Epub 2009/10/20. S0092-8674(09)01105-2 [pii] doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.08.035 19837040; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2780032.

28. Cervantes-Sandoval I, Martin-Pena A, Berry JA, Davis RL. System-like consolidation of olfactory memories in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 2013;33(23):9846–54. Epub 2013/06/07. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0451-13.2013 23739981; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3733538.

29. Trannoy S, Redt-Clouet C, Dura JM, Preat T. Parallel processing of appetitive short- and long-term memories in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2011;21(19):1647–53. Epub 2011/10/04. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.032 21962716.

30. Huang C, Wang P, Xie Z, Wang L, Zhong Y. The differential requirement of mushroom body alpha/beta subdivisions in long-term memory retrieval in Drosophila. Protein Cell. 2013;4(7):512–9. Epub 2013/06/01. doi: 10.1007/s13238-013-3035-8 23722532; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4875512.

31. Perisse E, Yin Y, Lin AC, Lin S, Huetteroth W, Waddell S. Different kenyon cell populations drive learned approach and avoidance in Drosophila. Neuron. 2013;79(5):945–56. Epub 2013/09/10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.045 24012007; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3765960.

32. Qin H, Cressy M, Li W, Coravos JS, Izzi SA, Dubnau J. Gamma neurons mediate dopaminergic input during aversive olfactory memory formation in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2012;22(7):608–14. Epub 2012/03/20. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.014 22425153; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3326180.

33. Waddell S, Armstrong JD, Kitamoto T, Kaiser K, Quinn WG. The amnesiac gene product is expressed in two neurons in the Drosophila brain that are critical for memory. Cell. 2000;103(5):805–13. Epub 2000/12/15. S0092-8674(00)00183-5 [pii]. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)00183-5 11114336.

34. Wu CL, Shih MF, Lai JS, Yang HT, Turner GC, Chen L, et al. Heterotypic gap junctions between two neurons in the Drosophila brain are critical for memory. Curr Biol. 2011;21(10):848–54. Epub 2011/05/03. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.02.041 21530256.

35. Wu CL, Shih MF, Lee PT, Chiang AS. An octopamine-mushroom body circuit modulates the formation of anesthesia-resistant memory in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2013;23(23):2346–54. Epub 2013/11/19. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.056 24239122.

36. Takemura SY, Aso Y, Hige T, Wong A, Lu Z, Xu CS, et al. A connectome of a learning and memory center in the adult Drosophila brain. Elife. 2017;6. Epub 2017/07/19. doi: 10.7554/eLife.26975 28718765; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5550281.

37. Wu CL, Fu TF, Chou YY, Yeh SR. A single pair of neurons modulates egg-laying decisions in Drosophila. PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0121335. Epub 2015/03/18. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121335 25781933; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4363143.

38. Haynes PR, Christmann BL, Griffith LC. A single pair of neurons links sleep to memory consolidation in Drosophila melanogaster. Elife. 2015;4. Epub 2015/01/08. doi: 10.7554/eLife.03868 25564731; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4305081.

39. Keene AC, Stratmann M, Keller A, Perrat PN, Vosshall LB, Waddell S. Diverse odor-conditioned memories require uniquely timed dorsal paired medial neuron output. Neuron. 2004;44(3):521–33. Epub 2004/10/27. S0896627304006476 [pii] doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.006 15504331.

40. Keene AC, Krashes MJ, Leung B, Bernard JA, Waddell S. Drosophila dorsal paired medial neurons provide a general mechanism for memory consolidation. Curr Biol. 2006;16(15):1524–30. Epub 2006/08/08. S0960-9822(06)01707-6 [pii] doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.022 16890528.

41. Aso Y, Sitaraman D, Ichinose T, Kaun KR, Vogt K, Belliart-Guerin G, et al. Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. eLife. 2014;4. Epub 2015/01/03. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04580 25553800.

42. Vogt K, Aso Y, Hige T, Knapek S, Ichinose T, Friedrich AB, et al. Direct neural pathways convey distinct visual information to Drosophila mushroom bodies. Elife. 2016;5. Epub 2016/04/16. doi: 10.7554/eLife.14009 27083044; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4884080.

43. Shih HW, Wu CL, Chang SW, Liu TH, Sih-Yu Lai J, Fu TF, et al. Parallel circuits control temperature preference in Drosophila during ageing. Nat Commun. 2015;6:7775. Epub 2015/07/17. doi: 10.1038/ncomms8775 26178754; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4518306.

44. Yang CH, Shih MF, Chang CC, Chiang MH, Shih HW, Tsai YL, et al. Additive expression of consolidated memory through Drosophila mushroom body subsets. PLoS Genet. 2016;12(5):e1006061. Epub 2016/05/20. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006061 27195782.

45. Wu CL, Chang CC, Wu JK, Chiang MH, Yang CH, Chiang HC. Mushroom body glycolysis is required for olfactory memory in Drosophila. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2018;150:13–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.02.015 WOS:000431470600002. 29477608


Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS Genetics


2020 Číslo 8
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy Podcasty Doporučená témata Časopisy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#